Corpus Linguistics in Chinese Contexts
eBook - ePub

Corpus Linguistics in Chinese Contexts

Simon Smith, Bin Zou, Michael Hoey, Bin Zou, Michael Hoey

Share book
  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Corpus Linguistics in Chinese Contexts

Simon Smith, Bin Zou, Michael Hoey, Bin Zou, Michael Hoey

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Rapid advances in computing have enabled the integration of corpora into language teaching and learning, yet in China corpus methods have not yet been widely adopted. Corpus Linguistics in Chinese Contexts aims to advance the state of the art in the use of corpora in applied linguistics and contribute to the expertise in corpus use in China.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Corpus Linguistics in Chinese Contexts an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Corpus Linguistics in Chinese Contexts by Simon Smith, Bin Zou, Michael Hoey, Bin Zou, Michael Hoey in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Literature & Asian Literary Criticism. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2015
ISBN
9781137440037

1

Lexical Priming: The Odd Case of a Psycholinguistic Theory that Generates Corpus-Linguistic Hypotheses for Both English and Chinese

Michael Hoey and Juan Shao

1.1 Corpus linguistics at the crossroads

Corpus linguistic research has had an iconoclastic effect on traditional linguistic theories and descriptions. Intuitions about the language have been found to be untrustworthy for over 20 years (e.g. Sinclair, 1991; Stubbs, 1995), and, in the wake of the methodological innovations associated with discourse analysis and sociolinguistics, attention has decisively shifted from guesswork about what an idealized speaker can theoretically say to hard evidence about what many thousands of speakers and writers actually do say. Whereas linguistics was once, for many, virtually synonymous with the study of grammar, the study of lexis is now widely accepted to be as important as grammar, with the role of collocation repeatedly recognized not only as ubiquitous but as central to our understanding of the way language works (Sinclair, 1991, 2004; Stubbs, 1995, 1996; Partington, 1998; Hoey, 1997, 2003, 2005). The generative linguist’s identification of competence as key to understanding linguistic creativity has been replaced by the corpus linguist’s identification of performance as key to understanding linguistic fluency (e.g. the idiom principle proposed by Sinclair, 1991; pattern grammar proposed by Hunston & Francis, 2000).
The energy with which traditional ideas have been knocked down has not however been matched by equivalent energy in rebuilding theories, and where theories have been proposed, they have often been incomplete or unconvincing. Although, for example, Sinclair’s idiom principle articulates an important truth about the way we construct our utterances, it leaves unarticulated the mechanism whereby one might fall back onto grammar when idioms fail to serve the speaker’s communicative process; nor is it convincing about why we should, as children, develop a grammatical system as a security mechanism for when our idioms fail. (Sinclair & Mauranen, 2006, admittedly offer a subtler picture.) On the whole, corpus linguistics has either ignored theoretical questions or (less commonly) related itself to pre-existing theoretical perspectives that appeared capable of accommodating corpus linguistic findings, e.g. cognitive linguistics and connectionism (Ellis, 2005; Gries & Stefanowitsch, 2006). There is of course nothing wrong with the latter course of action, but it may be premature until we have explored further what a corpus-based theory might look like.

1.2 A theory for corpus linguistics

One theory that has been developed in response to the insights derived from corpus linguistics, and in particular the work of John Sinclair, is that of Lexical Priming (Hoey, 2003, 2004a, 2004b, 2005; Hoey et al., 2007). If in due course this theory proves to be inadequate, it is to be hoped that it will nevertheless encourage more corpus-driven theoretical formulation. Lexical Priming is unusual as a (corpus-driven) theory in that it builds both upon corpus linguistic analysis and upon long-standing psycholinguistic research into the ways word recognition may be accelerated or retarded by previous exposure to other words, research which is widely accepted by psychologists but apparently little-known amongst corpus linguists.
Much of the psycholinguistic literature used by linguists is arguably more linguistic than psychological. But there are two research threads in psycholinguistics that may be of relevance, that are perhaps more psychological than linguistic: work on semantic priming and work on repetition priming.1
In semantic priming experiments, informants are shown a word or image (referred to as the prime) and then shown a second word or image (referred to as the target word). The speed with which the target word is recognized is measured. Some primes appear to retard informants’ recognition of the target and others appear to accelerate informants’ recognition of the target. For example, the prime word wing may be shown to inhibit the recognition of the word pig but to accelerate the recognition of the word swan.
As Pace-Sigge (2013) shows, the semantic priming literature is extensive and has an important pre-history; his book should be consulted by those interested in the psycholinguistic background. What is offered in this chapter is simply an outline account designed to explain the psycholinguistic underpinning of lexical priming theory before we consider its applicability to Chinese. The pioneering work on semantic priming was conducted by Meyer and Schvaneveldt (1971); they were the first to show that priming was scientifically demonstrable. Their work was followed through by Shelton and Martin (1992) (among others), who appear to show that semantic priming only works when the priming word and the target are associated in the informant’s mind. McRae and Boisvert (1998) argue, however, that if the words in question have closely related meanings there will be a priming effect even without such association. It will be noted that the earliest of the papers we have just cited is now over 40 years old and the phenomenon has not been refuted, even if the constraints upon its operation are still under discussion. While semantic priming does not explain the phenomenon of collocation, and is not necessarily the product of collocation, it is reasonable to look for a common explanation of both phenomena. This we find in repetition priming.
Repetition priming is rather different from semantic priming, in that the prime and the target are identical. Experiments with repetition priming centre on exposing informants to word combinations and then, sometimes after a considerable amount of time and after they have seen or heard a great deal of intervening material, measuring how quickly or accurately the informants recognize the combination when they finally see or hear it again. For example, a listener may be shown the word ALARMING followed by the word SUNSHINE. A day later, if s/he is shown the word ALARMING again, the informant may be found to recognize SUNSHINE more quickly than other words equivalently positioned after ALARMING. The assumption in such a case must be that s/he would be remembering the combination from the first time, since a combination of words such as ALARMING SUNSHINE will only rarely have occurred before, and quite probably not in the earshot or reading of the informant, and is therefore unlikely to comprise a learnt expression.
Again, we refer the reader to Pace-Sigge (2013) for a fuller account of the relevant literature. Key papers on these facets of repetition priming, though, are those of Jacoby and Dallas (1981), who observed greater accuracy in the identification of the tar...

Table of contents