Interpretive Conventions
eBook - ePub

Interpretive Conventions

The Reader in the Study of American Fiction

Steven Mailloux

Share book
  1. 230 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Interpretive Conventions

The Reader in the Study of American Fiction

Steven Mailloux

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

In Interpretive Conventions, Steven Mailloux provides a general introduction to reader-response criticism while developing his own specific reader-oriented approach to literature. He examines five influential theories of the reading process—those of Stanley Fish, Jonathan Culler, Wolfgang Iser, Norman Holland, and David Bleich. He goes on to argue the need for a more comprehensive reader-response criticism based on a consistent social model of reading. He develops such a reading model and also discusses American textual editing and literary history.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Interpretive Conventions an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Interpretive Conventions by Steven Mailloux in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Literature & Literary Criticism Theory. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

CHAPTER ONE

Literary Theory and Psychological Reading Models

The Affective Fallacy is a confusion between the poem and its results (what it is and what it does). … It begins by trying to derive the standard of criticism from the psychological effects of the poem and ends in impressionism and relativism.
—Monroe C. Beardsley and W. K. Wimsatt, “The Affective Fallacy”
However disciplined by taste and skill, the experience of literature is, like literature itself, unable to speak.
—Northrop Frye, Anatomy of Criticism
American literary theory has seen an explosion of interest in readers and reading. There is talk of implied readers, informed readers, fictive readers, ideal readers, mock readers, superreaders, literents, narratees, interpretive communities, and assorted reading audiences. The term “reader-response criticism” has been used to describe a multiplicity of approaches that focus on the reading process: affective, phenomenological, subjective, transactive, transactional, structural, deconstructive, rhetorical, psychological, speech act, and other criticisms have been indiscriminately lumped together under the label “reader response.” In these first two chapters I will bring some order into this metacritical chaos by comparing the most prominent models of reading and the critical theories based on those models. To do this, I will investigate the work of the five reader-response critics who have been most influential in the United States: Stanley Fish, Norman Holland, David Bleich, Wolfgang Iser, and Jonathan Culler. Out of this investigation will come an agenda for developing a reader-oriented approach to American fiction study.

Reader-Response Criticism?

All reader-response critics focus on readers during the process of reading. Some examine individual readers through psychological observations and participation; others discuss reading communities through philosophical speculation and literary intuition. Rejecting the Affective Fallacy of American New Criticism, all describe the relation of text to reader. Indeed, all share the phenomenological assumption that it is impossible to separate perceiver from perceived, subject from object. Thus they reject the text’s autonomy, its absolute separateness, in favor of its dependence on the reader’s creation or participation. Perception is viewed as interpretive; reading is not the discovery of meaning but the creation of it. Reader-response criticism replaces examinations of a text in-and-of-itself with discussions of the reading process, the “interaction” of reader and text.
Stanley Fish’s early essay, “Literature in the Reader: Affective Stylistics” (1970) presented one influential version of this reader-response criticism. Fish viewed a sentence in the text not as “an object, a thing-in-itself, but an event, something that happens to, and with the participation of, the reader.” His claims were aggressively descriptive: “In my method of analysis, the temporal flow is monitored and structured by everything the reader brings with him, by his competences; and it is by taking these into account as they interact with the temporal left-to-right reception of the verbal string that I am able to chart and project the developing response.”1 And the developing response was that of the “informed reader,” a reader with the ability to understand the text and have the experience the author intended.2 In this “affective stylistics,” Fish talked as if a text manipulated the reader—the text forced the reader to perform certain cognitive acts—and Fish, as practical critic, described that manipulative process. As a critical theorist Fish attacked formalist approaches, especially American New Criticism, for ignoring “what is objectively true about the activity of reading.” He claimed that his own approach was in contrast “truly objective” because it recognized the “fluidity … of the meaning experience” and directed our attention “to where the action is—the active and activating consciousness of the reader.”3
In a major reversal, Fish rejects these claims in “Interpreting the Variorum” (1976), where he argues that all texts are in fact constituted by readers’ interpretive strategies and that the process he formerly claimed to describe is actually a creation of his critical theory: “What my principles direct me to ‘see’ are readers performing acts; the points at which I find (or to be more precise, declare) those acts to have been performed become (by a sleight of hand) demarcations in the text; those demarcations are then available for the designation ‘formal features,’ and as formal features they can be (illegitimately) assigned the responsibility for producing the interpretation which in fact produced them.”4 This radical revision of Fish’s theory has two consequences: a change in the relation of reader to text and a change in the relation of criticism to reading. Fish now claims that in reading the interpreter constitutes the text and that in reader criticism the interpreter’s description constitutes the nature of the reading process according to his interpretive strategies.
Fish has moved from a phenomenological emphasis (which describes the interdependence of reader and text) to a structuralist or even post-structuralist position (which studies the underlying systems that determine the production of textual meaning
fig0001
and in which the individual reader and the constraining text lose their independent status). In his metacriticism, Fish has given up making descriptive claims for his earlier critical approach and abandoned its absolute priority over formalist criticism. He now views affective stylistics as only one of many possible interpretive strategies; it does not describe how all readers read but instead suggests one way they could read. Though Fish now holds it to be an act of persuasion rather than objective description, he continues to use his earlier approach when he does practical criticism. He therefore occupies two places on the schema of reader-response criticism shown in the chart.
This schema locates each critic on a continuum of reader-oriented approaches. A detailed examination of these approaches will reveal not only the interrelations among the critics placed here but also the problems within each of their reader-response theories. All five critics construct a theory consisting (in more or less detail) of an account of interpretation, a model for critical exchange, and a model of reading. These critics’ theories of interpretation try to account for meaning-production in both reading and criticism. Their models of critical discussion specify the nature of critical procedures (observation, description, explication, and explanation) and the ways interpretations are exchanged in critical dialogue. These hermeneutic theories and critical models are based on models of reading, accounts of how readers actually interact with the text during the temporal reading process. Norman Holland’s work provides a useful starting point for the following discussions of reader-response criticism because his writings carefully examine all three of these components making up a critical theory.5

Transactive Criticism

Holland’s transactive criticism “takes as its subject-matter, not the text in supposed isolation, as the New Criticism claimed it did, nor the self in rhapsody, as the old impressionistic criticism did, but the transaction between a reader and a text.”6 The notion of an “identity theme” is central to Holland’s approach: “we can be precise about individuality by conceiving of the individual as living out variations on an identity theme much as a musician might play out an infinity of variations on a single melody.” A person brings this “unchanging inner core of continuity” to all transactions between Self and Other, including reading.7
Holland’s model of reading proceeds from his more general theory of the relation between personality and perception. Perception is a “constructive act,” not merely reflecting but forming reality: “the individual apprehends the resources of reality (including language, his own body, space, time, etc.) as he relates to them in such a way that they replicate his identity.”8 That is, perception is also interpretation, and “interpretation is a function of identity, specifically identity conceived as variations upon an identity theme.” Holland particularizes this view of perception in his central thesis about reading: identity re-creates itself. “All of us, as we read, use the literary work to symbolize and finally to replicate ourselves. We work out through the text our own characteristic patterns of desire and adaptation.”9
Within this principle of identity re-creation, Holland isolates four specific modalities, which he conveniently organizes under the acronym DEFT—-defenses, expectations, fantasies, and transformations. “One can think of these four separate principles as emphases on one aspect or another of a single transaction: shaping an experience to fit one’s identity and in doing so, (D) avoiding anxiety, (F) gratifying unconscious wishes, (E) absorbing the event as part of a sequence of events, and (T) shaping it with that sequence into a meaningful totality.”10 The concept of a “meaningful totality” or unity is pivotal for Holland’s reading model (and is equally important in his general theory of interpretation).11 According to Holland, the reader makes sense of the text by creating a meaningful unity out of its elements. Unity is not in the text but in the mind of a reader. “By means of such adaptive structures as he has been able to match in the story, he will transform the fantasy content, which he has created from the materials of the story his defenses admitted, into some literary point or theme or interpretation.”12 For Holland, meaning is the result of this interpretive synthesis, the transformation of fantasy into a unity which the reader finds coherent and satisfying. As with all interpretation, “the unity we find in literary texts is impregnated with the identity that finds that unity.” Each reader creates a unity for a text out of his own identity theme, and thus “each will have different ways of making the text into an experience with a coherence and significance that satisfies.”13 Therefore, Holland’s model of reading accounts exceptionally well for varied responses.
On the other hand, Holland’s present theory has trouble with the phenomenon of similar responses. Similarity was easily explained by his earlier model. In The Dynamics of Literary Response (1968), he spoke as if “fantasies and their transformations were embodied in the literary work, as though the work itself acted like a mind”; different readers could take in (“introject”) the same text and “participate” in whatever psychological process was embodied there. Accounting for recurrent responses has become much more difficult in Holland’s revised model, in which “processes like the transformation of fantasy materials through defenses and adaptations take place in people, not in texts.”14 No longer embodying psychological processes, autonomous texts no longer serve as a guarantee of recurrence in Holland’s present model of reading. Instead, similar identity themes must somehow account for similar response.15 This psychological explanation contrasts with Fish’s “sociological” ones. In Fish’s earlier theory, all informed readers had the same basic reading experience because they shared linguistic and literary competence; in his present theory, communal reading strategies account for similar interpretive responses. The comparison between Fish’s and Holland’s reading models becomes more complex when we examine the precise status of the text in their revised theories.16
Holland and Fish both claim that perception is a constructive act: we interpret a...

Table of contents