Mindfulness in Social Psychology
eBook - ePub

Mindfulness in Social Psychology

Johan C. Karremans, Esther K. Papies, Johan C. Karremans, Esther K. Papies

Compartir libro
  1. 168 páginas
  2. English
  3. ePUB (apto para móviles)
  4. Disponible en iOS y Android
eBook - ePub

Mindfulness in Social Psychology

Johan C. Karremans, Esther K. Papies, Johan C. Karremans, Esther K. Papies

Detalles del libro
Vista previa del libro
Índice
Citas

Información del libro

Scientific interest in mindfulness has expanded in recent years, but it has typically been approached from a clinical perspective. This volume brings recent mindfulness research to classic social psychology topics such as romantic relationships, prejudice, prosocial behavior, achievement, and self-control. Written by renowned scholars in social psychology, it combines a comprehensive research overview with an in-depth analysis of the processes through which mindfulness affects people's daily life experiences. It provides theoretical and methodological guidance for researchers across disciplines and discusses fundamental processes in mindfulness, including its effect on emotion regulation, executive control, automatic and deliberative processing, and its relationship to self-construal and self-identity. This book will be of particular interest to upper-level students and researchers in social psychology, health psychology, and clinical psychology, as well as social work and psychology professionals.

Preguntas frecuentes

¿Cómo cancelo mi suscripción?
Simplemente, dirígete a la sección ajustes de la cuenta y haz clic en «Cancelar suscripción». Así de sencillo. Después de cancelar tu suscripción, esta permanecerá activa el tiempo restante que hayas pagado. Obtén más información aquí.
¿Cómo descargo los libros?
Por el momento, todos nuestros libros ePub adaptables a dispositivos móviles se pueden descargar a través de la aplicación. La mayor parte de nuestros PDF también se puede descargar y ya estamos trabajando para que el resto también sea descargable. Obtén más información aquí.
¿En qué se diferencian los planes de precios?
Ambos planes te permiten acceder por completo a la biblioteca y a todas las funciones de Perlego. Las únicas diferencias son el precio y el período de suscripción: con el plan anual ahorrarás en torno a un 30 % en comparación con 12 meses de un plan mensual.
¿Qué es Perlego?
Somos un servicio de suscripción de libros de texto en línea que te permite acceder a toda una biblioteca en línea por menos de lo que cuesta un libro al mes. Con más de un millón de libros sobre más de 1000 categorías, ¡tenemos todo lo que necesitas! Obtén más información aquí.
¿Perlego ofrece la función de texto a voz?
Busca el símbolo de lectura en voz alta en tu próximo libro para ver si puedes escucharlo. La herramienta de lectura en voz alta lee el texto en voz alta por ti, resaltando el texto a medida que se lee. Puedes pausarla, acelerarla y ralentizarla. Obtén más información aquí.
¿Es Mindfulness in Social Psychology un PDF/ePUB en línea?
Sí, puedes acceder a Mindfulness in Social Psychology de Johan C. Karremans, Esther K. Papies, Johan C. Karremans, Esther K. Papies en formato PDF o ePUB, así como a otros libros populares de Psychology y History & Theory in Psychology. Tenemos más de un millón de libros disponibles en nuestro catálogo para que explores.

Información

Editorial
Routledge
Año
2017
ISBN
9781317238829
Edición
1
Categoría
Psychology

1

Why social psychologists should care about mindfulness

Johan C. Karremans and Esther K. Papies

Why social psychologists should care about mindfulness

Over the past 30 years, the concept of mindfulness has found its way into mainstream psychological science. Initially, research approached mindfulness mainly from clinical and neurocognitive perspectives, focusing on its potential stress-buffering effects and the prevention and reduction of depressive symptoms. Mindfulness, however, may have much broader implications, and these are increasingly recognized by scientists. As noted by Jon Kabat-Zinn, one of the pioneering scholars in this field, “mindfulness […] has profound relevance for our present-day lives” (1994, p. 3). Mindfulness not only may impact a general sense of well-being and health, but may affect daily activities like eating, sleeping, and learning; it may affect our emotions, our goals, and the decisions we make; and it may affect our sense of self and how we interact with and relate to other people.
Social psychology is the science of everyday human behaviour, and not surprisingly then, interest in mindfulness has started to increase among social psychologists as well. Yet, while articles on this topic appear regularly in the main social psychology journals, the concept is still studied by only a relatively small subgroup of social psychologists. In this chapter, we reach out to a broader audience and argue that the concept of mindfulness may have important implications for a wide variety of topics that are traditionally studied by social psychologists. We first provide a brief historical background, and we discuss what mindfulness is, and what it is not. Reviewing recent mindfulness research on social psychological topics, we then discuss how social psychology as a field could benefit from engaging with mindfulness for theory, empirical research, and applications. Finally, we will discuss how mindfulness researchers in turn could profit from integrating social psychological theory and methodology into their work. In doing so, we hope to inspire constructive cross-talk between the fields.

A brief history of mindfulness in Western science

The concept of mindfulness was introduced in Western psychology in the late 70s by Jon Kabat-Zinn, a medical scientist at the University of Massachusetts. Rooted in Buddhist contemplative traditions and teachings (for an extensive discussion, see Williams & Kabat-Zinn, 2013), Kabat-Zinn developed a secular mindfulness-training program. Using meditation exercises and psycho-education, participants gradually learn to stabilize their attention to increase moment-to-moment awareness of body sensations, thoughts, and emotions, and to approach these experiences non-judgementally and with curiosity. Initially developed to treat patients with chronic pain, over the years a large number of participants around the world have participated in mindfulness-based training programs for a wide variety of reasons, including anxiety, depressive symptoms, and stress; sleeping problems; rumination; impulsivity and aggressive tendencies; concentration problems; or simply for personal and spiritual growth.
In the wake of the growing popularity of these programs in Western society, a first wave of mindfulness research emerged. Clinical studies started to evaluate the effectiveness of the training programs, and of mindfulness meditation practice more generally, for issues such as depression relapse prevention (Teasdale, Segal, & Williams, 1995), the treatment of anxiety disorders (Miller, Fletcher, & Kabat-Zinn, 1995), and quality of life among chronic pain patients (Kabat-Zinn, 1982; Kabat-Zinn, Lipworth, & Burney, 1985). While the evaluation of mindfulness-based interventions with regard to individual health and well-being is still a rapidly expanding field of research, mindfulness-related techniques have already been incorporated into various forms of clinical practice (e.g. acceptance and commitment therapy, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy; see Baer, 2015).
Once support for the effectiveness of mindfulness in promoting psychological well-being had accumulated, a second wave of inquiry – roughly the past 15 years – concentrated on the more specific question of how the effects of mindfulness emerge. For example, neuroscientists and cognitive psychologists started to examine the cognitive and neural underpinnings of mindfulness, finding evidence that mindfulness is associated with increases in executive control (Teper, Segal, & Inzlicht, 2013; see also Elkins-Brown, Teper, & Inzlicht, Chapter 5 in this volume), attentional control (Chambers, Lo, & Allen, 2008), and structural changes in brain areas associated with such functions (e.g. Davidson et al., 2003; Hölzel et al., 2011; Zeidan et al., 2011). Moreover, as we will discuss in this chapter, researchers began paying increased theoretical and empirical attention to the specific psychological mechanisms that may be associated with mindfulness, such as changes in emotion regulation and empathy (e.g. Goldin & Gross, 2010; Birnie, Speca, & Carlson, 2010, respectively), and changes in perspectives on the self (see Leary & Diebels, Chapter 4 in this volume).

What is mindfulness – and what is it not?

But what exactly is mindfulness? Although a variety of definitions have been suggested, researchers most commonly define mindfulness as a state of paying conscious attention to present-moment experiences with an open and non-judgemental attitude (Kabat-Zinn, 1990). In this definition, two components can be distinguished: 1) focusing attention on present-moment experiences, including bodily sensations, thoughts, and emotional states, and 2) approaching these experiences with a non-judgemental attitude, irrespective of their valence (Bishop et al., 2004). Thus, being mindful means observing one’s immediate and current experiences, and acknowledge them for what they are in this present moment, or put differently, giving them bare attention (Epstein, 2008).1
Although this may sound relatively simple and easy to do, most people who have attempted to train this skill, through meditation or mindfulness training, have quickly found that it can be quite difficult. While important individual differences exist (see for example Alberts, Chapter 2 in this volume), various domains of research suggest that for many people a state of mindful awareness is not something that occurs naturally, nor often, in daily life. Some would even say that most of the time, people are and act in a state of mindlessness. For example, research on automaticity suggests that significant portions of our daily activities are guided by unconscious and automatic processes (Wyer, 2014). Moreover, the mind has an extremely strong tendency to wander, and without realizing it, people are typically engrossed in thoughts about the past or future, rather than the present moment, relating their experiences to their self-concepts (Farb et al., 2007) and making them unhappy (Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010). In addition, as soon as negative emotions or difficulties occur, many people have an automatic tendency to avoid or suppress the experience, turning their attention away from it (Gross & John, 2003). On top of this, people often judge, approach, and avoid objects and other people automatically, as social psychologists have convincingly shown (e.g. Chen & Bargh, 1999; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995; Herring et al., 2013).
All of these examples of mindlessness can be contrasted with a state of mindfulness, in which a person attends to and becomes aware of internal experiences and automatic response tendencies; the mind is not wandering but focused on the present moment; and one is turning attention towards experiences – whether negative, neutral, or positive – receptive to whatever is going on in mind and body, with an attitude of acceptance and non-judgement. In other words, a state of mindfulness can be described as a state in which one takes a decentered meta-cognitive perspective on one’s current-moment experiences, including one’s thoughts and feelings about the self, rather than immediately responding to them (e.g. Bishop et al., 2004). To give an example: One may observe that, at this very moment, there is an emotional tone of anger, that there is tension in the body, that there are thoughts about revenge, and perhaps behavioural inclinations to aggress. Instead of immersing oneself in these experiences, in a mindful state, one’s perspective shifts from “within one’s subjective experience onto that experience” (p. 599; Bernstein et al., 2015), which may fundamentally change how these experiences affect us, and may provide one with more ‘freedom’ of how to respond next.
Before we start discussing how this all may be applied to social psychological theory and research, it is helpful to consider briefly how mindfulness can be distinguished from related concepts, particularly those that have been studied extensively in social psychology (for a more extensive discussion, see Brown & Ryan, 2003). First, some of these concepts also entail attention and awareness, most notably self-monitoring and self-awareness. Self-monitoring refers to the capacity to observe and evaluate one’s behaviour against a set of standards or norms (Snyder, 1974), allowing one to adjust one’s behaviour accordingly. Relatedly, self-awareness refers to the ability to recognize one’s feelings, behaviours, and traits, and evaluate and compare them to internal standards (Wicklund, 1975). While there is some overlap of these concepts with mindfulness in that the focus of attention is on internal experiences, mindfulness critically differs from them as it not concerned with standards or norms. It entails the observing of direct experience not through an evaluative lens, without trying to understand the experience or having the immediate intention of changing it. Second, while mindfulness requires the regulation of attention, and mindfulness can promote successful self-regulation (see Elkins-Brown et al., Chapter 5 in this volume), it should not be equated with self-regulation or self-control. Whereas self-control entails the active down-regulation of emotions or impulses, mindfulness means to simply observe them as they are, with no other goal than simply observing – even though almost paradoxically, this often facilitates their regulation. Finally, it is important to mention that the concept of mindfulness discussed in this volume differs from Langer’s conceptualization of mindfulness (Langer, 1989), which refers to mindfulness as the ability to “notice new things”, not determined by old routines or rules, when paying full momentary attention to one’s surroundings. While the concepts overlap in the sense that they both entail paying attention with an open and curious mind, the concept of mindfulness we refer to is concerned with non-judgemental, meta-cognitive attention to the nature of one’s momentary experiences (not the external world per se).
These differences from self-related concepts typically studied by social psychologists reflect the fact that while psychological research traditionally is concerned with studying the consequences of particular contents of consciousness (e.g. certain biases, specific thoughts, specific emotions, and so forth), mindfulness is concerned with the nature of people’s cognitive processes and experiences, rather than their specific content. As a result, mindfulness as an intervention technique is also distinct from typical emotion regulation techniques like reappraisal, which focus on changing the content of one’s thoughts or experiences. Instead, mindfulness is concerned with changing how one perceives and relates to the contents of consciousness (e.g. experiencing them as ‘real’ vs. accepting them as mere mental events). As will be discussed in greater detail later in this chapter, this can have important implications for how one is affected by and responds to thoughts and emotions.

The value of mindfulness for social psychological research

What is the potential value of studying mindfulness for understanding everyday human behaviour? Why should social psychologists care about mindfulness? We propose that mindfulness is relevant for social psychologists because it has implications for social psychological theory, particularly where automaticity and self-relatedness are concerned, and it has important implications for applications to positively affect people’s lives.

Implications for social psychological theory

As noted by Barsalou (Chapter 3 in this volume), mindfulness’s distinct focus on attention to internal experiences has important consequences for the interplay of automatic and controlled processes. Mindfulness also directly affects self-related processes (e.g. Leary & Diebels and Elkins-Brown et al., Chapters 4 and 5 in this volume). These basic constructs – automaticity and self – are central to most social psychological theories or models, and are crucial to understanding real life human behaviour.

Mindfulness and automatic versus controlled responding

The brain’s capacity to regulate behaviour automatically and unconsciously is highly adaptive. Habitual and automatic response patterns can be extremely powerful in guiding us through life in a relatively effortless manner, allowing us to respond accurately and without much deliberation to the social and non-social environment (e.g. Custers & Aarts, 2010). At the same time, however, habitual and mindless responding may be at the root of various problems and challenges an individual may encounter in life. As discussed by Barsalou (Chapter 3 in this volume), life-long conditioned responses often remain unattended and outside of conscious awareness, while such responses may not necessarily be most effective in terms of increasing one’s own well-being, or the well-being of relationships with others.
Typically, psychological theories assume that an automatically triggered behavioural response will be enacted. However, theorizing and research in mindfulness shows this link can be broken if attention is directed purposefully at the behavioural impulses themselves. Indeed, one of the central ideas of mindfulness is that it increases awareness of impulses, and while accepting these experiences as being merely transient mental events, an individual is able to prevent automatically acting on them, and can reconsider how to respond most effectively to his or her environment. In other words, mindfulness points to the potential for controlled processes to regulate automatic processes in novel ways – not by focusing on the content of thought, but by directing attention to their nature as mere mental events.
The chapters in this volume offer several examples of how mindfulness can reduce automatic responding and thus affect daily life outcomes. Papies (Chapter 7 in this volume) provides an overview of research suggesting that mindfulness can affect health-relevant behaviour, including healthy eating, smoking, and alcohol use. To explain such findings, she discusses how mindfulness promotes the monitoring of automatic impulses and cravings that often play a critical role in unhealthy behaviour. Becoming consciously aware of such impulses is a first prerequisite to reduce the otherwise automatic link between impulse and behavioural response (e.g. mindlessly lighting a cigarette when the impulse arises; mindlessly emptying a bag of potato chips in a habitual snacking situation). Importantly, however, a second prerequisite for not reacting to the impulse is to observe it from a non-judgemental and decentered perspective, which often allows the impulse to dissipate before it turns into actual behaviour.
Karremans and Kappen (Chapter 8 in this volume) discuss how a similar process may occur in the context of close relationships. Again, smooth interactions between partners may be guided by habitual responses, but certain habitual patterns may be the cause of relationship trouble. For example, as major social-psychological theories in relationship science recog...

Índice