Erving Goffman and Modern Sociology
eBook - ePub

Erving Goffman and Modern Sociology

Philip Manning

Share book
  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Erving Goffman and Modern Sociology

Philip Manning

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

The work of Erving Goffman has had an enormous impact throughout the social sciences. Yet his writings have not received the detailed scrutiny which they deserve.

This new book is the first comprehensive and accessible account of Erving Goffman's contributions, ranging in its scope from his very earliest work right up to the projects upon which he was engaged at the time of his death. Goffman's writings, Manning argues, are much more systematic and conceptually powerful than is ordinarily acknowledged. The book thus offers a defence of Goffman's writings as well as providing an introduction for those who have no prior acquaintance with Goffman's ideas.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Erving Goffman and Modern Sociology an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Erving Goffman and Modern Sociology by Philip Manning in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Social Sciences & Sociology. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Polity
Year
2013
ISBN
9780745667669
Edition
1

1

Introduction and Overview

INTRODUCTION
In this book I present a brief, but comprehensive, account of the ideas of Erving Goffman (1922–82), and show why these ideas are central to modern sociology. The idea that Goffman is central to sociology is mildly ironic, because he is often remembered as an outsider, a brilliant maverick, a one of a kind genius, a man who is “bleakly knowing” about modern urban life. From this view, he is a dispassionate observer who sees through our day-to-day performances and self-presentations. For example, Gary Marx tells us that “Goffman presented himself as a detached, hard-boiled cynic, the sociologist as 1940s private eye. His was a hip, existential, cool, personal style” (1984: 637). Although there is truth to this view, it does have one limitation: it plays down Goffman’s commitment to the development of sociology. His acute observations about everyday life were not only meant to make us think again about our day-today behavior; they were also part of an abstract analysis of social interaction. For many years Goffman tried to develop a general theory of face-to-face interaction, a theory that could be used to interpret any social exchange, whether it took place in a bar or a boardroom. However, despite his enthusiasm for this general theory, he also remained extremely skeptical about the possibility of discovering such a general theory. It is as if he were committed to finding this theory at one moment and indifferent to it at the next.
For his supporters, it is the tension between his attempt to develop a theory of social interaction and his doubts that such a theory exists that makes his work so provoking. For his critics, this tension makes Goffman inconsistent, and his books hard to interpret. Often Goffman seems to have two contradictory “voices”: one voice tells of a general pattern beneath different examples of ordinary behavior, while the other emphasizes the crucial differences between examples.
Although Goffman’s attempt to develop a general theory of face-to-face interaction is fascinating in its own right, it also raises a question. Is it possible for sociologists to develop a general sociological theory, a theory both of institutions and social interaction? In his introduction to Frame Analysis, Goffman wrote about the limits of his own work while supporting the ambitious projects pursued by other sociologists:
I make no claim to be talking about the core matters of sociology – social organization and social structure. 
 I am not addressing the structure of social life but the structure of experience individuals have at any moment of their social lives. I personally hold society to be first in every way and any individual’s current involvements to be second. (1974: 13)
In the final chapter I look at one attempt to develop a general theory of “the structure of social life” using Goffman’s ideas. This is the structuration theory developed by Anthony Giddens. This theory thrusts Goffman to the center of contemporary debates about the relationship between structure and agency. In the later chapters in this book I suggest that in order to theorize agency it is necessary to understand the use of rules in everyday life. I suggest that Goffman offers an incomplete account of rule-following, and that his ideas have to be supplemented with insights from ethnomethodology. I do not see Goffman as a precursor to ethnomethodology; rather, I believe that a combination of his ideas with theirs provides an important resource for mainstream sociology. I also believe that Giddens’s theory is the best available vehicle for their delivery.
There are many ways of describing Goffman: he can be seen as a one of a kind observer, a cynic, an ethnographer, a symbolic interactionist. In this book, I portray Goffman as someone with competing visions, as someone who sees both cynical manipulation and trust in social interaction, as someone who sees sociology as both cumulative science and as humanistic enquiry, as someone who sees his own work as both a loose set of acute insights and as an organized description of the basic elements face-to-face interaction.
However, this is in the future. In the rest of this chapter I play the book as a whole on fast forward, anticipating and sketching the ideas that will be considered in greater detail in subsequent chapters.
Goffman was primarily an observer of face-to-face interaction who possessed an extraordinary ability to appreciate the subtle importance of apparently insignificant aspects of everyday conduct. Goffman made his readers aware of this almost invisible realm of social life, with the result that the banal exchanges and glances observable in any public place become a continual source of fascination. With the exception of Simmel’s, his general descriptions of face-to-face interaction are unmatched.
Let me begin with a biographical preliminary. Born in Canada in 1922, Goffman obtained a BA degree in sociology and anthropology from the University of Toronto in 1945. A chance meeting with Everett Hughes persuaded him to move to Chicago for graduate work, which led to a doctoral thesis about social interaction on a small island community off the coast of Scotland. The thesis was actually written in Paris, where he was exposed to the then fashionable doctrine of existentialism. After completing his Ph.D. at the end of 1953, and unable to obtain a tenure track position, he worked for Edward Shils on a project concerning social stratification. During this time he wrote the first edition of The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. In 1956, a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health allowed him to move to Washington, DC to study the experience of inmates at a large urban mental hospital. In 1961, Herbert Blumer invited him to join the sociology faculty at the University of California at Berkeley. After six intellectually successful years at Berkeley, he spent a transitional year as a Visiting Fellow at Harvard, before moving to the University of Pennsylvania in 1968, where he remained until his death of cancer in 1982, at the age of 60.
Throughout his writings Goffman worked to develop a vocabulary that could describe the general features of face-to-face interaction. Although this was primarily a problem of description, it also meant that he had to explain the motivations behind everyday behavior. As a result his work contains many “how” and “why” accounts. For example, he discussed both the odd behavior of people in elevators, describing the way they stare at their feet or the floor numbers above the door and the reasons why this behavior is important to others sharing the elevator. We can understand his work as a kind of map to the uncharted world of everyday life. Goffman saves us from over-familiarity, allowing us to see the complexity, stability, and importance of apparently mundane social interaction.
Curiously, we are quite unable to explain how or even why we do most of the things that we do with supreme practical ease in our daily lives. Whether walking down a street or answering a phone call, the way we perform these activities is both more intricately patterned and more important than most of us could believe possible. In our daily lives we often act on autopilot: we comply with a set of implicit instructions that govern our behavior. Social life is patterned because we often choose to follow these instructions and thereby make the world predictable. Predictability is an astonishing collective accomplishment.
Even subtle departures from such patterns can undermine our confidence that the social world is as it appears to be. Departures are understandable if they replace the prevailing definition of the situation with another; they are destructive if they cannot be interpreted. One of the major legacies of Goffman’s work is that it shows us how the fragility of day-to-day life is lent solidity and order by small gestures and ritual offerings. Many of the details of face-to-face encounters (which we frequently fail to notice) reappear in his books as examples of how our trust in an otherwise unruly environment is maintained. For example, when a man apologizes for inadvertently stepping in front of someone in a queue, it is easy to miss the way he touches her elbow as he speaks; but it is this physical contact that assures her of the sincerity of the apology.
With the help of many examples Goffman identified the factors that underpin our confidence in the social world. He typically convinces us of this by suggesting scenarios in which that world suddenly becomes quite alien and startling. For example, when we pass pedestrians in the street, we routinely establish eye contact for a moment, only to then look away. This gesture is a ritual courtesy that affirms respect among strangers in a small and almost unnoticeable way. However, to maintain that eye contact for even an extra few seconds before looking away transforms a gesture of support into a hostile act: it becomes a “hate stare” of the sort practiced against Blacks in America’s deep South in the 1950s (Goffman, 1963: 83).
By scratching at the surface of many of our day-to-day routines, Goffman uncovered a machinery of social interaction. However, it would be wrong to use this metaphor to suggest that we are just inanimate cogs; or to put the matter in a more contemporary idiom, just hardware for interactional programs that churn out a predictable and generally safe social world. On the contrary, Goffman was fascinated by our reflexive ability to manipulate these procedures for social interaction. Even if we often choose to live on autopilot, casually allowing the predictable flow of social interaction to guide us through a host of encounters, we are able to switch to manual control in order to establish our autonomy. For example, we are generally unaware of how we are sitting; nevertheless, when circumstances demand (perhaps an important interview), we are able to spend a great deal of time worrying about whether we are sitting in an appropriate way. Goffman was very alert to such occasions; in his expression, he remained “lovingly empirical”. He made the mundane world refreshingly new. Unlike the traditional anthropologist who broadens our horizons by expanding our knowledge of other societies, Goffman shows us the complexity of our own.
Although there are divergences and shifting points of emphasis, it is true to say that Goffman’s corpus remained focused on the study of the minutiae of social life. Unlike so many great figures in the Arts and Social Sciences, such as Marx, Wittgenstein, and Heidegger, there is not a great discrepancy between Goffman’s early and late work. Instead we find continued attempts to analyze everyday phenomena and a curious willingness to abandon apparently apt ideas or “tools”.
Unsurprisingly, his writings have had a profound influence on most of the social sciences, but what is surprising is that the initial excitement is often short-lived. In part, this is one of the paradoxes of success: his ideas seem so well known, his books so well read, that it is redundant to comment on them. Goffman has become an overfamiliar landmark on the intellectual map of modern social science, a person who is cited everywhere but rarely discussed in detail. Nearly all social scientists can say something about “total institutions” and the “art of impression management” but few are able to give a comprehensive account of his ideas. At best, social scientists discuss the fragment of his work that bears substantively on their own interests. This may explain why there is no “Goffman School” to continue his work.
A SUMMARY OF GOFFMAN’S IDEAS
In his Master’s thesis at Chicago, submitted in 1949, there is a protracted and ultimately vain attempt to use statistics to understand an audience’s responses to a then popular American radio soap opera called “Big Sister”. This work is quite unlike his later studies: it is dry, almost unauthored, and his only statistically based project. A couple of years later, in a study for the American Petroleum Institute, he studied the aspirations and frustrations of service-station dealers (people who either owned or leased petrol stations). For this, he used both quantitative and qualitative data, thereby bridging his earlier methodology with his developing interest in ethnography.
Prior to his dissertation, Goffman also published two papers, the first concerning the ways in which status symbols can be “borrowed” by those without the means to obtain them legitimately, the second about the extent to which everyday life is experienced as a huge confidence trick, in which we are all “marks” needing to be “cooled out”. These two papers are a drastic departure from the quantitative approach of his Master’s thesis, and the first signs of his distinctive writing style.
For his dissertation, which he was awarded at the end of 1953, Goffman spent eighteen months on a small island off the Scottish coast, studying face-to-face interaction in a crofting community. Alleging to be an American (he was to the end a Canadian) interested in agricultural techniques, Goffman studied the ways in which the islanders disclosed and hid information from each other. Working as a part-time assistant washer-up, he observed their everyday rituals and practices close up. With only 300 families on a small, flat island with almost no vegetation to partition the landscape, the crofters lived in almost continual sight of one another, and they became for Goffman a microcosm of society. With an elaborate set of definitions, classifications, and examples, he developed his field notes into a general account of face-to-face interaction. In contrast to the dry text of his Master’s thesis, this work contains extended metaphors and sharp touches. Goffman returned to these ideas frequently in the ensuing years.
In 1956 the University of Edinburgh published his first book, The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life, which discussed a number of “dramaturgical principles”. Each of these principles explores the social world as if it were a theatrical performance. The social world becomes filled with “impression management” and team performances, as each of us is transformed into a cynical role player who hides behind an array of performance masks. This image sharpens one contained both in parts of his doctoral thesis and in his earlier paper on the ways in which life is experienced as a confidence trick. Three years later, in 1959, The Presentation of Self was reissued by Penguin Press in Great Britain and by Anchor in the States. What had begun life as a relatively obscure research monograph was destined to become a bestseller. There are several changes to the second edition, which superficially appear only to amplify the initial themes. However, on closer inspection they have a corrosive quality, and ultimately undermine the coherence of the view that we are all cynical actors performing instrumentally for personal gain. Retrospectively, these appended passages to the second edition mark a subtle but important change in Goffman’s ideas.
In the late 1950s and 1960s, Goffman pursued three interdependent projects, all involving the interpretation of everyday behavior. First, he produced work that appears to fall squarely into the sociology of deviance. In 1961, his extraordinary ethnography, Asylums, was published. This analyzes the experiences of inmates in a Washington mental institution, and by extension, in any institution in which the time and space of subordinates are carefully monitored and restricted. He called these places “total institutions”, and his discussion of them helps us to understand the apparently irrational behavior of those who are held there. The four essays that make up this book combine extensive field observations with a dense knowledge of the scientific and popular literature. The result is an ethnography that is less a study of a specific hospital and more an ethnography of the concept of the total institution itself.
Goffman’s second contribution to the sociology of deviance was called Stigma (1964a). Beginning with a poignant story of the hardships faced by a girl born without a nose, this book suggests that we are all stigmatized in some way: we are too fat, too short, we are losing our hair, we have failed in some activity in which we have invested a degree of self worth, etc. As a result, we have all learned to manage discrediting information about ourselves.
His second major project of the 1960s parallels his earlier work on the theatrical metaphor, only on this occasion he considered the social world not as theater but as a game. In Encounters (1961b) and Strategic Interaction (1970), he discusses everyday meetings as interactional “moves” enacted by players. This work leads Goffman to the edges of game and rational-choice theory; indeed, some of this work dates from his year spent with Thomas Schelling at Harvard. With characteristic imagination, Goffman draws upon an unusual source – true and fictional espionage stories – as a way of challenging our sense of what the mundane world is like. In this vein, he tells us that although to us a wrong number may be only a minor disruption to our day, to a spy it may cause anxiety and fear of disclosure. Our security in the fabric of the social world is not a natural feature of that world, but stems instead from a generally held set of rules of behavior. If we all lived our daily lives as spies, the social world would seem a frightening environment, where every meeting was a potential cause for fear. Of course, it is possible to see this erosion of trust in parts of cities such as New York. The key point is that we view these and similar locations as disturbing exceptions rather than as the norm. If the anxieties felt by many people in some parts of New York were typical of modern life, our experience of the social world would be radically different.
The third of Goffman’s projects in the 1960s was the specification of the assumptions people use as they take part in social interaction. For most of us most of the time, the social world is predictable and routine. It is essential to see that this predictability is neither natural nor assured; instead it is an astonishing collective achievement to which we contribute daily in myriad ways. This means that the explanations of why, for example, kettles boil and cars stop at traffic lights must be quite separate. Goffman was quietly appalled by the number of methodologies that assume that people are comparable to inanimate objects. In the preface to Relations in Public (1971), for example, he suggests that sociologists who concentrate on hypothesis-testing are engaging in a type of “sympathetic magic” from which no knowledge of ordinary behavior can be gleaned. This type of research, he suggests, employs natural science analogs with only the hope that science will result (1971: 21).
Instead of viewing the predictability of social life as the consequence of underlying laws, Goffman thought that predictability was something that we all made happen; it is then, an issue of rule-following behavior. These rules of social interaction do not produce social order (they do not compel us to act); rather they are a way of exhibiting social order. Rules are subject to interpretation, to disagreement about what constitutes an occasion of rule-following, to exceptions, and to decisions not to abide by them. That the world is even in the slightest a predictable place is an extraordinary and largely invisible accomplishment.
Throughout the 1960s Goffman spent a considerable amount of time attempting to specify the broad assumptions people use in their everyday lives. By tracing a hesitant path through several of his books, it is possible to discern four basic assumptions, which I refer to as his SI AC schema. It is important to remember that Goffman did not use this acronym.
The first element of SI AC is “situational propriety”. This suggests that the meaning of our actions is linked to the context in which they arose, and that we can rarely understand behavior without knowledge of the situation in which it occurred. If I ask someone to marry me, the significance of this act means one thing in a church and another in a theater.
Buried in his account is a critique of the mainstream American psychiatric practice of the 1950s, which had, Goffman thought, replaced a physiological explanation of different mental illnesses with a non-medical classification of socially unacceptable behavior. Goffman thought that situational improprieties reveal the structural obligations of social interaction. They uncover mixed motives, sometimes pointing to absent-mindedness, sometimes to feelings of alienation, frustration, antagonism, resentment, social incompetence, and sometimes to mental illness. Behavior which appears to indicate “madness” can on other occa...

Table of contents