Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning
eBook - ePub

Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning

Angela M. O'Donnell, Alison King, Angela M. O'Donnell, Alison King

  1. 376 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning

Angela M. O'Donnell, Alison King, Angela M. O'Donnell, Alison King

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

The contribution of this volume to the literature on peer learning is its focus on approaches that reflect a common concern with cognitive processes based in developmental, information processing, or more generally, constructivist perspectives on peer learning. Although the clear importance of the social context of peer learning is not ignored, the volume's emphasis is on the cognitive growth that occurs within the learning environment. Any discussion of peer learning involves consideration of who is learning, how the role of peers with whom one works can be conceptualized, what it is that peers learn together, what changes as a result of the interaction, and how we can know what occurs in groups or what has been learned. The chapters in this book speak to these questions. The key question underlying many of these others is why we should worry about the intricacies of peer interaction. Both the practical and theoretical reasons for doing so are delineated. The developmental theory presented in the Introduction lays the foundation for the later descriptions of specific techniques, though many of the techniques reflect a range of other influences as well. Part I presents the implications of the work of two major theorists in cognitive development, Piaget (Ch. 1) and Vygotsky (Ch. 2). In Part II, six chapters describe a variety of peer learning techniques or models of collaboration, many of which are influenced by the work of Piaget and Vygotsky. The chapters in Part III consider the role of the teacher and the skills needed when using peer learning as an instructional strategy. The Conclusion points to areas in which further research is needed. This volume is based on original papers presented by the contributing authors in November 1996 at the Rutgers Invitational Symposium on Education on Cognitive Skills and Learning With Peers.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on ā€œCancel Subscriptionā€ - itā€™s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time youā€™ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlegoā€™s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan youā€™ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, weā€™ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning by Angela M. O'Donnell, Alison King, Angela M. O'Donnell, Alison King in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Education & Education General. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2014
ISBN
9781135688189
Edition
1

PART II

Cognitiveā€“Elaborative Approaches to Peer Learning: Overview

The six chapters in this part describe a variety of techniques that depend to a greater or lesser extent on cognitive elaboration theory (Slavin, 1995). Such approaches are based on general information processing theory and suggest that in order to remember information and connect it to existing knowledge, a student must restructure the information or elaborate on the information (Wittrock, 1978, 1990). The chapters also draw on the cognitive-developmental theories of Piaget and Vygotsky described in Part I of this book. In addition, many of the chapters in this part are strongly influenced by sociocultural perspectives on learning and often reflect cognitive-constructivist perspectives.
The chapters in this Part II describe specific techniques or models of collaboration. Three of the chapters (chaps. 3, 4, & 5) specifically address the quality of discourse within a peer learning group and issues related to providing effective support for productive interaction. Two additional chapters (chaps. 6 & 7) extend this discussion. The last chapter in this part provides a commentary on these chapters. Because the various chapters included here describe specific techniques, we thought it important to end this part of the book with a chapter that provided some integration of the other chapters. The final chapter by Derry draws attention to the underlying themes or ideas that are common to these chapters.

CHAPTER THREE

Evolution of Discourse During Cross-Age Tutoring
NATALIE K. PERSON
Rhodes College
ARTHUR G. GRAESSER
University of Memphis
It has been well documented that one-to-one tutoring is an effective method of instruction. Reported effect sizes have ranged from 0.4 to 2.3 standard deviation units when tutored students are compared to classroom instruction or other control groups (Anderson, Corbett, Koedinger, & Pelletier, 1995; Bloom, 1984; Cohen, Kulik, & Kulik, 1982; Mohan, 1972). These effect sizes are quite surprising considering that a normal tutor typically has minimal tutoring experience, lacks expert domain knowledge, and has received no training in sophisticated tutoring techniques (Fitz-Gibbon, 1977; Graesser & Person, 1994). Although the majority of studies that report the benefits of tutoring primarily focused on outcomes, some of the more contemporary research is aimed at understanding the tutoring process (Fox, 1991, 1993; Graesser, Bowers, Hacker, & Person, in press; Graesser & Person, 1994; Graesser, Person, & Magliano, 1995; Lepper, Aspinwall, Mumme, & Chabay, 1990; Merrill, Reiser, Ranney, & Trafton, 1992; Person, Graesser, Magliano, & Kreuz, 1994; Putnam, 1987). Many of these researchers have attempted to understand the effectiveness of normal, unskilled tutors by systematically analyzing the collaborative dialogue that occurs between tutors and students.
The purpose of this chapter is to illustrate the pedagogical mechanisms that facilitate learning during normal one-to-one tutoring interactions. We believe that the advantages that one-to-one tutoring has over other learning methods are best understood by analyzing the contributions that tutors and students make while attempting to answer a question or solve a problem. In a number of previous publications and manuscripts, we documented the pedagogical tactics and strategies that tutors frequently implement during these interactions. We also provided substantial evidence that tutors rarely employ sophisticated tutoring techniques that are often incorporated into structured tutoring facilities in school systems and intelligent tutoring systems on computers (Graesser et al., in press; Graesser & Person, 1994; Graesser et al., 1995; Person, 1994; Person et al., 1994). In this chapter, we illustrate some of our previous claims about tutor strategies via actual tutoring dialogue excerpts. The chapter contains four sections. We first briefly describe the two tutoring samples that we used in our analyses. We then discuss the five-step dialogue frame that is prevalent in tutoring sessions but not in classrooms. A general understanding of the five-step frame is important because many of our subsequent arguments are housed within this framework. The third section provides a brief overview of sophisticated strategies that are rarely implemented by normal tutors. The last section illustrates the pedagogical strategies and dialogue moves that normal tutors did use in our tutoring samples.
TWO TUTORING SAMPLES
Tutoring protocols were collected from two naturalistic tutoring samples. The students in the first sample consisted of 27 undergraduates enrolled in a psychology research methods course who were required to participate in the tutoring sessions in order to fulfill a course requirement. The instructor of the research methods course identified six topics that are typically difficult for students to master. These topics included (a) operationally defining variables, (b) interpreting graphs, (c) statistics, (d) designing an experiment to test a hypothesis, (e) factorial designs, and (f ) interpreting interactions. A counterbalancing scheme ensured that each student attended two different tutoring sessions with two different tutors.
The tutors for the research methods sample were three psychology graduate students who were recommended by a faculty member and were considered to be competent in the area of research methodology. Each of the tutors had some prior tutoring experience but not in the area of research methods.
The students in the second sample consisted of 13 seventh graders who were having difficulty in algebra and who were recommended for tutoring by their algebra instructors. They were tutored on topics such as fractions, exponents, positive and negative numbers, and algebra word problems. This sample included all of the algebra tutoring that took place at this particular middle school during a 1-month period. Therefore, it should be noted that this sample is representative of the tutoring that takes place in school settings.
The tutors for the algebra sample were 10 high school students who had performed well in previous mathematics courses. None of the tutors in these two samples had extensive tutoring experience (approximately 9 hours each), had ever received formal training in tutoring strategies, or could be considered experts in their respective domains. Therefore, these tutors were typical of the tutors who typically work with students in school settings. The algebra tutors all attended the same school as the seventh-grade algebra students and were roughly 3 years older than the tutees.
The tutors and students in the two samples, however, should not be considered peers in the sense that their level of expertise is fundamentally equal. Other researchers have investigated learning situations in which two peers with roughly equal knowledge collaborate to solve a complex task (Oā€™Donnell, Dansereau, Hythecker, et al., 1988; Oā€™Donnell et al., 1990; Rogoff, 1990). Our tutors are not peers in this sense.
All tutoring sessions were videotaped and transcribed. The research methods sample had 44 hours of tutoring, whereas the algebra sample had 22. Some of the sessions in the research methods sample could not be transcribed because of poor sound. A full account of our data collection, transcribing procedures, coding methods, and statistical analyses can be found in several previous articles (Graesser & Person, 1994; Graesser et al., 1995; Person et al., 1994; Person, Kreuz, Zwaan, & Graesser, 1995). Our previous analyses indicated virtually no differences between the two samples in terms of the dialogue patterns, pedagogical strategies, and tactics that were employed by the tutors. Therefore, we do not attempt to differentiate these two samples in our illustration of such patterns, tactics, and strategies.
THE FIVE-STEP DIALOGUE FRAME
Some researchers have characterized classroom interactions in terms of a three-step dialogue frame. The sequence of these three steps involves: (1) the teacherā€™s request for information from a student, (2) the studentā€™s answer, and (3) the teacherā€™s evaluation of the studentā€™s contribution. Mehan (1979) referred to these three steps as initiation, response, and evaluation, whereas Sinclair and Coulthart (1975) referred to them as question, answer, and evaluation. Regardless of the labels (IRE or QAE), neither of these three-step frames requires extensive input from the student.
The dialogue frame that is prevalent in tutoring consists of five steps rather than three. Our previous publications provide detailed descriptions of the five steps (Graesser & Person, 1994; Graesser et al., in press; Graesser et al., 1995). A brief description of the five steps follows:
1. Tutor asks a question (or alternatively provides a problem for the student to solve).
2. Student answers the question.
3. Tutor gives feedback on the answer.
4. Tutor and student collaboratively improve the quality of answer.
5. Tutor assesses studentā€™s understanding of the answer.
The five-step dialogue frame is illustrated in the following exchange from a tutoring session on the topic of factorial designs (research method).
Example 1
Step 1
1:1
TUTOR:
So, how many F scores would be computed?
Step 2
1:2
STUDENT:
Three.
Step 3
1:3
TUTOR:
Three [agreeing with the student].
Step 4
1:4
TUTOR:
And what numbers [referring to a matrix of cell means] would you use?
1:5
STUDENT:
You would do one for humor [one of the independent variables].
1:6
TUTOR:
And what does that tell you?
1:7
STUDENT:
Iā€™m not sure [laughs].
1:8
TUTOR:
OK, why do you do an F score? What is an F score?
1:9
STUDENT:
To see the size, uh, significance?
1:10
TUTOR:
The size of significance.
1:11
STUDENT:
The size of significance.
1:12
TUTOR:
Right, how statistically significant a variable is.
1:13
STUDENT:
Right.
1:14
TUTOR:
So, you are right, you would have three [F scores]: one for caffeine, one for humor, and one for . . . ?
1:15
STUDENT:
The scores . . . from caffeine and humor.
1:16
TUTOR:
Interaction, the interaction of the two, right?
1:17
STUDENT:
Um hmm.
1:18
TUTOR:
[Explains independence of main effects].
Step 5
1:19
TUTOR:
Do you see what Iā€™m saying?
1:20
STUDENT:
Um hmm.
The nature of this dialogue is quite different from what typically occurs in classroom interactions. Notice that Step 4 consists of multiple turns that allows the tutor and student to elaborate on ideas raised in the earlier steps. It would be quite difficult for a teacher in a classroom to craft such an elaborative dialogue that would meet the needs...

Table of contents

Citation styles for Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning

APA 6 Citation

[author missing]. (2014). Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning (1st ed.). Taylor and Francis. Retrieved from https://www.perlego.com/book/1554522/cognitive-perspectives-on-peer-learning-pdf (Original work published 2014)

Chicago Citation

[author missing]. (2014) 2014. Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning. 1st ed. Taylor and Francis. https://www.perlego.com/book/1554522/cognitive-perspectives-on-peer-learning-pdf.

Harvard Citation

[author missing] (2014) Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning. 1st edn. Taylor and Francis. Available at: https://www.perlego.com/book/1554522/cognitive-perspectives-on-peer-learning-pdf (Accessed: 14 October 2022).

MLA 7 Citation

[author missing]. Cognitive Perspectives on Peer Learning. 1st ed. Taylor and Francis, 2014. Web. 14 Oct. 2022.