The Routledge Companion to Trust
eBook - ePub

The Routledge Companion to Trust

Rosalind H. Searle, Ann-Marie I. Nienaber, Sim B. Sitkin, Rosalind H. Searle, Ann-Marie I. Nienaber, Sim B. Sitkin

  1. 588 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

The Routledge Companion to Trust

Rosalind H. Searle, Ann-Marie I. Nienaber, Sim B. Sitkin, Rosalind H. Searle, Ann-Marie I. Nienaber, Sim B. Sitkin

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

In recent years, trust has enjoyed increasing interest from a wide range of parties, including organizations, policymakers, and the media. Perennially linked to turbulence and scandals, the damaging and rebuilding of trust is a contemporary concern affecting all areas of society.

Comprising six thematic sections, The Routledge Companion to Trust provides a comprehensive survey of trust research. With contributions from international experts, this volume examines the major topics and emerging areas within the field, including essays on the foundations, levels and theories of trust. It also examines trust repair and explores trust in settings such as healthcare, finance, food supply chains, and the internet.

The Routledge Companion to Trust is an extensive reference work which will be a vital resource to researchers and practitioners across the fields of management and organizational studies, behavioural economics, psychology, cultural anthropology, political science and sociology.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is The Routledge Companion to Trust an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access The Routledge Companion to Trust by Rosalind H. Searle, Ann-Marie I. Nienaber, Sim B. Sitkin, Rosalind H. Searle, Ann-Marie I. Nienaber, Sim B. Sitkin in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Business & Business General. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2018
ISBN
9781317595700
Edition
1

Part I
Foundations of trust

The foundations part includes four chapters which examine six key topics that are important for the field. In the first of our foundation chapters, Daan van Knippenberg examines affect-based trust, a topic which is neglected in many studies, leading to it having an underestimated role in trust. He makes the case for it being distinct from cognition-based trust, considering its unique antecedents and consequences. Important here, and why we deliberately position this as our first foundation chapter, is the contention that affect-based trust has a stronger influence on trust than cognitive-based trust. This chapter is concerned not merely with emotions but with relationships.
Then in Chapter 2 M. Audrey Korsgaard reviews the interpersonal concept of reciprocal trust, a complex, bidirectional phenomenon which involves a self-reinforcing dynamic process. Korsgaard develops further the importance of relationships, which van Knippenberg puts central as a foundation, to consider three key facets of reciprocal trust: trust spirals and their directions, the dynamics of relationships and the shapes of the trust trajectory.
This is followed by Kirsimarja Blomqvist and Karen S. Cook who focus on swift trust, updating a concept which Meyerson, Weick and Kramer (1996) introduced. In their chapter, as well as testing some of the founding propositions, they look at new organizational phenomena and specifically at the role of technology and virtual interactions to explore knowledge-creation activities, in which trust might be required to form rapidly and has to deliver immediate benefits.
Sim B. Sitkin and Katinka M. Bijlsma-Frankema in Chapter 4 take us to the debate of trust versus distrust. Their chapter considers two distinct ways that distrust has been approached: as a subset of trust and in its own right as a distinct phenomenon. They review the evidence to identify further directions that arise from these different perspectives for the field.

1
Reconsidering Affect-Based Trust

A new research agenda

Daan van Knippenberg
It is hard to envision social relationships that would not require some level of trust between parties to function effectively. Regardless of whether these are relationships between individuals, group members, organizations or linkages across those levels (e.g. individual–employing organization), regardless of whether these are work or non-work relationship, regardless of whether these are short-term or longer-term relationships, arguably all relationships build on trust between parties to be effective (cf. Fulmer & Gelfand, 2012). Not surprisingly then, trust is a concept studied widely in the social and behavioural sciences (Rousseau, Sitkin, Burt & Camerer, 1998). The jumping off point for the current discussion of trust is that in this impressive body of work there is such an emphasis on trust as a cognitive phenomenon that the role of affective processes in trust is under-investigated and, I would contend, underestimated. The idea that one would trust, or distrust, someone because one has “a good feeling” about the person, or a “bad feeling” about the person seems completely natural; yet, this idea is essentially absent from the empirical work on trust.
Following the widely used definition by Rousseau et al. (1998) I understand trust to be the willingness to accept vulnerability based upon positive expectations about another’s behaviour (key here is that vulnerability entails risk, and trust thus captures the willingness to expose oneself to that risk). Expectations are cognitions, and it is therefore probably not surprising that trust research has been strongly dominated by a cognitive approach to trust in which trust is understood as the outcome of a subjectively rational cognitive process. This is illustrated in influential conceptual treatments of trust (e.g. Mayer, Davis & Schoorman, 1995) and in the observation that most empirical work tends to focus on cognition-based trust (e.g. Dirks & Ferrin, 2002). Even so, affect – moods and emotions – may play a role in trust. In trust research, this is captured first and foremost by the concept of affect-based trust, trust based on the personal bond between two people (McAllister, 1995; Webber, 2008). Despite the fact that affect-based trust is in a sense a “minority perspective” in trust research, there is enough work on affect-based trust to take stock of its contribution to the trust literature. The aim of this chapter is to do exactly that, and to review what we know from empirical research on affect-based trust to assess the case for a unique role of affect within the greater body of trust research – is there evidence of unique antecedents? Of unique consequences? The review aims to not merely “sum up” the evidence, but to arrive at both integrative conclusions and identify a way forward for research in affect-based trust – a research agenda.
Presaging things to come, the first-blush conclusion would be that affect-based trust and cognition-based trust are moderately to strongly correlated, but do represent different constructs with unique antecedents and consequences. Moreover, there is evidence that affect-based trust tends to be the stronger influence and that cognition-based trust can be seen as a precursor to affect-based trust. On closer consideration, however, one may spot the elephant in the room: affect-based trust does not so much invoke affect as it invokes relationship; it is relationship-based trust much more than affect-based trust. From its definition as trust based on the personal bond between two people, onwards it is arguably better labelled relationship-based trust than affect-based trust in that the relationship is definitional but affect is not. In that sense the label affect-based trust is something of a misnomer, and the primary driver of the difference between affect-based and cognition-based trust may not be so much the contrast between affect and cog nition but the extent to which the trust is relationship-based or reflects an assessment of the other party’s trustworthiness that is relatively independent of the relationship between parties. Recognizing this important alternative understanding of the affect-based–cognition-based trust distinction, I propose that research in affect-based trust does not, or at the very least not un equivocally, address the role of affect in trust. The key conclusion I would advance, therefore, concerns the need for research on the one hand to clarify the relationship basis versus affective basis of affect-based trust, and on the other hand to unambiguously study the role of affect in trust.

Affect-based trust: state of the science

As outlined by Lewis and Weigart (1985) and Mayer et al. (1995), our assessment of the extent to which some other party (person, group, organization) can be trusted has a strong component of subjective rationality. Trust follows from what one sees as good reasons – indicators of a party’s trustworthiness. Schoorman et al. (1995) for instance argue that the key indicators of trustworthiness concern the other party’s competence, integrity and benevolence. As noted by Lewis and Weigart (1985) and McAllister (1995), however, the quality of the relationship between parties and its emotional connotations can also be a basis for trust, and this has led to the rise of the concept of affect-based trust (as I will argue below, a misnomer for what conceptually and empirically is relationship-based trust). In research in management, McAllister (1995) captured this probably in the most influential way by explicitly distinguishing and operationalizing cognition-based trust and affect-based trust. In this understanding of trust, the key antecedent of affect-based trust is the quality of the relationships between parties. Indeed, operationally McAllister’s measure of affect-based trust reflects judgment of relationship quality as much as the willingness to be vulnerable in the relationship (i.e. only the latter matches the more common definition of trust) and does not have the notion of affect as a basis for trust as a main theme underlying the items. Cognition-based trust, in contrast, would be based primarily on more factual indicators of a party’s competence, integrity and benevolence (i.e. feeding into expectations of what the party is able and motivated to do; cf. Mayer et al., 1995).
Affect is clearly implied by the quality of a relationship, but I argue that this is a limited and limiting view of the role affect may play in trust, and moreover one that does not unambiguously speak to the role of affect but rather puts the quality of the relationship and not affect centre-stage. Before we get to that argument, however, I first provide a concise review of research on affect-based trust. The review aims to be representative, not exhaustive. To reflect my misgivings about the construct naming, I will refer to the construct as “affect-based trust”, that is, within quotation marks to indicate I am quoting the label used but not sharing the conclusion that the label is correct.

“Affect-based trust”: empirical evidence

“Affect-based trust” is conceptualized as rooted in the quality of the relationship between parties. By implication, cognition-based trust would not be, or to a lesser degree, rooted in this relationship. When cognition-based trust is understood as rooted in assessment of the other party’s competence, integrity and benevolence (Mayer et al., 1995), it seems reasonable to conclude that these are assessments that need not be based on the quality of the relationship with the other party (even when a relationship might give one access to relevant information pertaining to competence and integrity). For instance, we may assess someone’s competence on the basis of their CV, or judge a person’s integrity based on information available through others without any relationship with that person. In that sense, cognition-based trust could be said to be person-based – in contrast to the relationship-based nature of the concept known as “affect-based trust”. Cognition-based trust can thus be a precursor to “affect-based trust” to the extent that cognitive judgments of the other party’s trustworthiness may set the stage for the development of a high-quality relationship or for the absence of such a development.
Consistent with this logic, McAllister (1995) found support for a model in which relationship quality was a unique antecedent of “affect-based trust” in peers, and cognition-based trust too was an antecedent of “affect-based trust” (and “affect-based trust” predicted performance). Other studies also report such evidence of cognition-based trust as an antecedent of “affect-based trust”. Schaubroeck, Peng and Hannah (2013) for instance found that cognition-based trust in leader and peers predicted “affect-based trust”, which in turn predicted identification and performance. Newman, Kiazad, Miao and Cooper (2014) observed that ethical leadership predicted cognition-based trust, which in turn predicted “affect-based trust”, which predicted organizational citizenship behaviour. Other research shows that such findings generalize beyond inter personal relationships at work, such as for consumer trust (Johnson & Grayson, 2005), inter-organizational trust (Robson, Katsikeas & Bello, 2008; see also Dowell, Morrison & Heffernan, 2015) and trust in virtual communities (open source software development; Stewart & Gosain, 2006).
Research also corroborates McAllister’s (1995) argument and findings that “affect-based trust” and cognition-based trust have unique antecedents and consequences. Chua, Ingram and Morris (2008) for instance found that “affect-based trust” and cognition-based trust have different types of social network ties as precursors, and Dunn, Ruedy and Schweitzer (2012) show that “affect-based trust” suffers from upward comparison to the other party (i.e. because it puts one in an unfavourable light) whereas downward comparison is bad for cognition-based trust (i.e. because it implies lower competence).
Other research shows that “affect-based trust” and cognition-based trust have (partially) different consequences. Ergeneli, Ari and Metin (2007) found for instance that cognition-based trust predicted the competence and meaning aspects of psychological empowerment, whereas “affect-based trust” predicted the impact aspect of empowerment. Ng and Chua (2006) found that “affect-based trust” was positively related to cooperation whereas cognition-based trust was not. Ha, Park and Cho (2011) found that “affect-based trust” was related to information sharing and benefit/risk sharing, whereas cognition-based trust was related to joint decision making and benefit/risk sharing. Chua, Morris and Mor (2012) found that cultural meta-cognition was positively related to “affect-based trust” (but not cognition-based trust) in cross-cultural relationships. Taking a moderator perspective Parayitam and Dooley (2009) studied conflict and trust in strategic decision-making teams and found that cognition-based trust was a moderator in the relationship between conflict and outcomes whereas “affect-based trust” did not moderate the relationship.
A series of studies also speaks to different roles for “affect-based trust” and cognition-based trust in leadership. Yang, Mossholder and Peng (2009) observed that cognition-based trust mediated the relationship of supervisory procedural justice with performance and job satisfaction, whereas “affect-based trust” mediated the relationship between supervisory procedural justice and helping behaviour at work. In a different spin on the mediating role of these two types of trust for justice effects, Colquitt, LePine, Piccolo, Zapata and Rich (2012) showed that “affect-based trust” mediated the social exchange-based (i.e. relationship-based) effects of organizational justice, whereas cognition-based trust mediated the uncertainty-reducing effects of justice. Miao, Newman, Schwarz and Xu (2013) reported that “affect-based trust” mediated the relationship between participative leadership and organizational commitment whereas cognition-based trust did not have a mediating role. Schaubroeck, Lam and Peng (2011) found that servant leadership influenced team performance through “affect-based trust” and team psychological safety. Transformational leadership (a concept that has been invalidated by conceptual and empirical problems; van Knippenberg & Sitkin, 2013) influenced team performance through cognitive trust. Zhu, Newman, Miao and Hooke (2013) found that “affect-based trust” mediated the relationship between transformational leadership and commitment, performance and organizational citizenship behaviour, whereas cognition-based...

Table of contents