Personality Theories
eBook - ePub

Personality Theories

Development, Growth, and Diversity

Bem P. Allen

Share book
  1. 550 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Personality Theories

Development, Growth, and Diversity

Bem P. Allen

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

This text provides a comprehensive introduction to the key personality theorists by combining biographical information on each theorist with his or her contributions to the field, including her or his ranking among the world's most respected psychologists. In addition, Allen provides a tabular formatā€“that is, a running comparison between the major theorists, allowing students to analyze new theories against theories learned in previous chapters. The unique style of Allen's book is strengthened through his conversational tone, enabling students to easily grasp an understanding of the key people and movements in the field of personality.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on ā€œCancel Subscriptionā€ - itā€™s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time youā€™ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlegoā€™s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan youā€™ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, weā€™ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Personality Theories an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Personality Theories by Bem P. Allen in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Psychology & Personality in Psychology. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2015
ISBN
9781317351139
Edition
5

CHAPTER 1

Introduction

ā€¢ How is personality defined and measured?
ā€¢ Are personality researchers scientists?
ā€¢ What kinds of tests do personality psychologists use?
One goal of this first chapter is to answer the question ā€œWhat is personality?ā€ by providing a preliminary definition. Another is to consider how personality is studied and the kinds of tests personologistsā€”personality psychologistsā€”use. A final goal is to lay out the logic behind the structure of chapters.

Preliminary Definition of Personality

The preliminary definition of personality has several facets: individual differences, behavioral dimensions, and traits. Individual differences refers to the observation that people differ in a variety of ways. In the study of personality, the important differences involve personality traits, internally based psychological characteristics that often correspond to adjectives such as shy, kind, mean, outgoing, dominant, and so forth.
Each trait corresponds to one end of a behavioral dimension, a continuum of behavior analogous to a yardstick. Just as one end of a yardstick is anchored by 0 inches and the other end by 36 inches, one end of a behavioral dimension is anchored by one behavioral extreme and the other end by the opposite extreme. For example, affiliativeness, anxiousness, and conscientiousness are labels for behavioral dimensions. The following example shows the extremes for conscientiousness:
conscientious:1:2:3:4:5:6:7:unconscientious
One end of the assertiveness dimension is anchored by conscientious, the tendency to be neat, well organized, on time, efficient, and effective. The other end is anchored by unconscientious, the tendency not to defend oneā€™s rights and to say ā€œyesā€ when one wants to say ā€œno.ā€ For the sake of convenience and simplicity, the example has only seven degrees. In reality, the number of degrees of a dimension is difficult or impossible to determine. In any case, only behaviors falling near the extremes of dimensions have much meaning for personality. Only if a personā€™s behavior can be represented on the assertiveness dimension by a degree close to the anchor assertive can it be inferred that he or she possesses the trait conscientious. A trait dimension may be seen as the internal representation of a behavioral dimension.
Putting the facets together, an individualā€™s personality is a set of degrees falling along many behavioral dimensions, each degree corresponding to a trait. Note that, although some people may share a particular trait, there are individual differences in possession of the trait. Some people have it and others do not. However, sharing does not apply to entire personalities. There are individual differences in the sets of traits that people possess such that no two personalities are exactly alike. Figure 1.1 shows the personality of Oprah, a favorite of students. The line drawn from each degree (trait level) to each other degree is her personality profile. Given enough dimensions, every personā€™s profile will be different from that of every other person. The line connecting degrees on dimensions is the ā€œpersonality profile line.ā€ For the middle-of-the-scale entries, Oprah is not extreme enough to be ā€œtraitedā€ on these dimensions. Thus, she has neither trait represented by the ends of these dimensions.

Implications and Cautions

There is a coincidence of personologistsā€™ beliefs about personality and the common sense notions held by nonprofessionals or laypersons. First, many personologists and laypersons believe that an individual is quite consistent across different situations (his or her behavior is at about the same point or degree along the assertiveness behavioral dimension in one situation as it is in the next). This point of view constitutes the most basic assumption of our preliminary definition of personality. Second, where single dimensions are involved, individuals can be very similar or even identical. That is, two or more people can, on the average, exhibit about the same degree of a behavior, and, thus, possess the same trait. Third, the overriding agreement between personologists and the people they study is the shared belief in individual differences (Lamiell, 1981). Because there are individual differences along every behavioral dimension, given enough dimensions, personalities must differ. One respected estimate puts the number of traits and corresponding dimensions at over 17,000, thereby guaranteeing that each individualā€™s personality is different from that of each other person and, thus, that each person has a unique personality (Allport and Odbert, 1936).
Unfortunately, some people mindlessly assume that ā€œindividual differencesā€ are immutable. If we view our positions on various dimensionsā€”whether they are personality or intellectual dimensionsā€”as ā€œinnateā€ and, thus, assumed to be unchangeable, our lives will be very limited. In an insightful article, Robert A. Bjork (2000) challenges the assumption that individual differences are set in stone. Basically, he takes on the widely held belief that we have or do not have valued characteristics the day we are born. That is, we tend to assume that our behaviors fall at the positive or the negative end of various behavioral dimensions, and, if the latter, that there is nothing we can do about it. If, according to one of Bjorkā€™s examples, we fail a standardized math test early in elementary school, we assume that we are no good at math and should give up on it. ā€œThe role of [innate] aptitude is over-appreciated and the role of experience, effort, and practice are under-appreciatedā€ (p. 3). We assume that we cannot, by our own devices, develop science aptitude, conscientiousness or the ability to appreciate othersā€™ emotions, so we donā€™t try. Further, if we do assume that our behaviors occupy the ā€œgoodā€ end of important dimensions, we are careful not to accept challenges, risk mistakes, or ā€œthink outside the boxā€ lest we disprove our assumption that we are inherently gifted. Iā€™ve known some honors students who assumed they were inherently smart, then avoided challenges and ā€œout of the boxā€ thinking lest they prove their assumption wrong. The result was mediocre work. I would add to Bjorkā€™s points that the burning need to know may be more important to mastering some pursuit than all the ā€œinnate intelligenceā€ we can claim for ourselves. Murray (2002) echos Bjorkā€™s rejection of over-attention to ā€œnatural abilityā€ and inattention to experience.
image
FIGURE 1.1 Oprahā€™s Personality
My experience as a personality researcher, reviewer of articles submitted to personality journals, and reader of personality publications tells me that the preliminary definition is implicitly, if not explicitly, adopted by more personologists than any other. It fits the theories of Murray, Cattell, Eysenck, and Allport rather well. All of them to some degree, at least implicitly, adopt its most basic assumption: a person behaves consistently from one situation to the next. These ā€œtrait theoristsā€ must make the consistency assumption. Otherwise they would not be able to infer a trait from observations of a personā€™s behaviors. They reason if a person is assertive in one situation and not in the next, how could anyone draw an inference about whether he or she has the trait conscientious?
The consistency assumption, while widely adopted, is nevertheless not without its critics. Some theorists, such as Rotter and Mischel, reject it outright. I also have serious reservations about it. Other deficiencies of our preliminary definition include inattention to physiological and developmental processes. In view of these shortcomings, consider the preliminary definition to be a frame of reference against which definitions of the theorists covered in this book can be considered. You will find that each theorist has her or his own definition, in some cases fitting the preliminary definition fairly well, but not always.

Methods of Studying Personality

First, a set of criteria for evaluating methods of studying personality must be specified. As in other fields of psychology, science is a main source of standards. The minimum requirement for a research method to be called scientific is that it involve unbiased observations that are quantified so that systematic analyses can be performed (Allen, 2001). Methods can properly be called ā€œscientificā€ if (1) they allow researchers to make observations without regard to their personal biases, and (2) they permit the assignment of numbers to those observations so that systematic analysis is possible. By contrast, users of unscientific methods base selection of observations on convenienceā€”whatever happens to be handy is observedā€”or personal biasā€”the researchers arbitrarily believe that selected observations are more important than other observations they might make. In addition, users of unscientific methods may draw conclusions about what they have observed without reference to theory or previous research. However important such efforts may be for some purposes, the methods involved do not ordinarily qualify as scientific.

The Case History Method

The case history method involves collecting background data about and making intensive observations of a single individual in order to discover how to treat that person or to obtain information that may apply to other people (Rosenhan & Seligman, 1995). It may be scientific in that observations can be unbiased. After all, it is possible for psychologists to lay aside their own personal biases about personality. However, unbiased observations may be difficult for them because the person under observation may be a patient with whom the psychologists are personally involved. Also, observations made on a single individual may not be applicable to other persons. A given individual may be quite unusual and, therefore, not at all representative of other people. Further, although it may be possible to assign numbers to observations, systematic analysis may be difficult or impossible. For example, a psychologist may give personality tests to an individual and then derive some scores. Still, systematic analysis may be difficult because scores from more than one person are ordinarily needed to conduct a meaningful analysis.
Despite these shortcomings, case histories may sometimes qualify as scientific. It is possible to give the subject of a case history personality tests and then compare her or his scores to norms or average scores derived by testing large populations of people. In this way, how the case history subjectā€™s scores compare to those of ā€œpeople in generalā€ can be determined and, thus, whether his or her score is unusual or like most peopleā€™s can be stated. Further, there have even been whole books written using a single case as the source of data for a statistical analysis in a scientific study (Davidson & Costello, 1969). Charles Potkay and I have actually used a single case to illustrate research results for our entire sample of research subjects (Allen & Potkay, 1973a, 1977).
While the case history method does not typically qualify as scientific, information derived from it may be useful in many ways. Reports based on observations of single individuals often are valuable as illustrations of personality functioning. While lamenting a lack of publications featuring case histories, Masling (1997) mused, ā€œā€¦ it is a pity, because a good, carefully written and clearly documented case can be more instructive, and usually more interesting, than any number of research papersā€ (p. 261).
At a number of points in this text, many real and some contrived cases will be used to concretely illustrate some aspect of personality functioning. Box 1.1 contains an example that relates to a criticism of the working definition of personality: personality is not set in stone; it can change. Bearing these cases in mind will help you remember some of the important patterns that relate to personality theories.
Among this bookā€™s case histories are some that you may find especially interesting: the Rat Man and the Wolf Man (and others, Chapter 2); characters in horror movies, delightful and mischievous Little Anna, and Bill Cosby (Chapter 3); an analysis of Frankenstein and his monster, based on the actual story by Mary Shelley (Chapter 4); the cases of Clare, a clingy, dependent woman madly in love with a self-centered man, and of the controller (Chapter 5); the good mother (Chapter 6); a hippie rebel turned conservative (Chapter 7); Hitler as a necrophilous (death-loving) character (Chapter 8); Becky and Dennis, Catholic and Protestant, coming to know one another in war-torn Northern Ireland (Chapter 9); a self-actualized person (Chapter 10); Jim and Joan, two college friends trying to resolve Jimā€™s conflict with a professor, and a cognitively simple person (Chapter 11); a person with an external orientation (Chapter 12); resilience in the face of defeat displayed by famous people (Chapter 13); a baby reared in a box (Chapter 14); dreams about the kidnapping and murder of the Lindbergh baby and an analysis of a disturbed college student (Chapter 15); a schoolgirl case of mass hysteria (Chapter 16); and the cases of Rinehart and Jenny and the mature person (Chapter 17). In addition, every theoristā€™s life story constitutes an often fascinating case history that begins every chapter.
In addition, your professor and you have a comprehensive case history in the Instructorā€™s Manual and in the Student Guide that accompanies Personality Theories (for the student guide see my Web site: www.wiu.edu/uĀ­sers/mfbpa/beĀ­mjr.html). The case history subject, Estella Monroe, is a young Latina mother who has survived a divorce and is ā€œmoving up the corporate ladder.ā€ Estellaā€™s relations with her son, her ex-husband, her new romantic partner, her coworkers, and her parents reveal material suitable for analysis by all the theories covered in this book.

BOX 1.1 ā€¢ George Foreman: From Angry and Depressed to Joyful and Successful

I remember the earlier George Foreman well. I was and am a big Muhammad Ali fan, but at the time of the ā€œrumble in the Jungleā€ (Zaire, Africa, 1974) Ali was past his prime and George Foreman was at his peak. Foreman appeared to be unbeatable. Joe Frazier had beaten Ali and Foreman ā€œk...

Table of contents