Conceptions of the Human Mind
eBook - ePub

Conceptions of the Human Mind

Essays in Honor of George A. Miller

Gilbert Harman, Gilbert Harman

Share book
  1. 296 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Conceptions of the Human Mind

Essays in Honor of George A. Miller

Gilbert Harman, Gilbert Harman

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

This volume is a direct result of a conference held at Princeton University to honor George A. Miller, an extraordinary psychologist. A distinguished panel of speakers from various disciplines -- psychology, philosophy, neuroscience and artificial intelligence -- were challenged to respond to Dr. Miller's query: "What has happened to cognition? In other words, what has the past 30 years contributed to our understanding of the mind? Do we really know anything that wasn't already clear to William James?" Each participant tried to stand back a little from his or her most recent work, but to address the general question from his or her particular standpoint. The chapters in the present volume derive from that occasion.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Conceptions of the Human Mind an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Conceptions of the Human Mind by Gilbert Harman, Gilbert Harman in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Psychology & History & Theory in Psychology. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2013
ISBN
9781134773855
Edition
1
1
The Implication of Specialized Neuronal Circuits versus Neuronal Number for Concepts Concerning the Nature of Human Conscious Experience
Michael S. Gazzaniga,
University of California, Davis
Sitting on top of the evolutionary process is the human brain. Its vast complexity, its capacity for complex problem solving, for inventiveness, for everything we know and appreciate about human cognition represents the finest achievement of the wondrous blind processes of selection that have gone on for millions of years. Many scientists have approached trying to gain clues to the understanding of the human brain’s special mechanisms of action by studying other species. Fair game for this enterprise includes comparisons between bugs and beasts of all types and kinds. Everything from our genetic mechanisms to the capacity to sleep, to feel, to remember, to transmit retinal information to visual cortex and a myriad of other processes have all been enlightened by careful animal experimentation over the years. Some have even seen in these studies so many similarities in neural structures that proposals are commonly put forward that animals have perceptual and cognitive process much like our own — indeed, a consciousness much like our own. Such views are usually qualified by the assertion that whatever differences do exist in conscious processes between species can be easily explained by the greater size of the human brain. Somehow having more neural cells is thought to produce a greater computational capacity that in turn yields that special quality of human conscious experience.
In fact, it is well known that the human brain is inordinately large after being corrected for body weight. Allometric considerations find the human brain falling off the correlation line assessing body size and brain size. This gigantic biologic organ weighing between 1,100 and 1,300 grams sits magnificently on top of a small body, guiding its sensations, actions, and desire for reproduction. These elements of human activity are indeed quite similar to those seen in both near and distant evolutionary relatives sharing many common brain mechanisms. Even basic dimensions of mental life, such as memory, attention, and visual perception, seem to share mechanisms and brain structures in common, certainly with other primates. As a consequence, the extra cortex in humans is viewed as critical for the higher cognitive functions we enjoy.
Yet, there is this puzzle. We humans do not seem to need great quantities of our huge cerebral cortex in order to carry out our intellectual activities. A cardinal feature of split-brain research, for example, is that following disconnection of the human cerebral hemispheres, the verbal IQ of the left hemisphere remains largely intact (Nass & Gazzaniga, 1987). Indeed, the problem-solving capacity of the left hemisphere remains unchanged (Le-Doux, Risse, Springer, Wilson, & Gazzaniga, 1977). Although there can be deficits in recall capacity (Phelps, Hirst, & Gazzaniga, 1992), by some performance measures the overall capacity to carry out problem solving seems unaffected. In other words, isolating essentially half of the cortex from the dominant left hemisphere causes no major change in intellectual function of the left hemisphere. This finding represents strong evidence that absolute cortical cell numbers have, at best, only a loose relation with human intelligence.
Related to the notion of special circuitry is the fact that disconnected right hemispheres are seriously impoverished in their ability to carry out a variety of computational tasks that the left would find easy to complete. In studies done some years ago in a small group of split-brain patients identified as having language capacities in each disconnected hemisphere, we carefully examined the problem-solving ability of the disconnected right and left hemispheres. Most commissurotomy patients do not have such abilities in their right hemisphere; correlated with that absence is an inability to carry out even the simplest perceptual and cognitive tasks. The small group we examined with language-capable right hemispheres were unable to make verbal inferences, solve spatial problems (Gazzaniga & Smylie, 1984), or generate indirect antonyms (Gazzaniga & Miller, 1989), to name a few of the tasks examined.
Although the right hemisphere remains superior for some activities such as the recognition of upright faces (Gazzaniga, 1989), some attentional skills (Mangun et al., 1992), and perhaps also emotional processes (Nass & Gazzaniga, 1987), it appears to be lacking in its overall cognitive capacity. In fact, it appears to be inferior to the mental capacities of a chimp.
The idea of the importance of specialized circuits is central to the field of human neuropsychology. Untold numbers of disease states have been reported that suggest there are specific disorders following focal lesions or long fiber-tract disconnections (Nass & Gazzaniga, 1987). Human brains seem to house a constellation of special-purpose networks that are given over to rather specific tasks. When that fact is considered in light of evolutionary processes, it is hard to imagine it could be any other way. Why would evolution leave to chance the building up of networks needed for each human to survive from scratch with each birth? Surely it is advantageous to be delivered into this world with as much original equipment as possible. Millions of years of evolution would allow for exactly this kind of thing; it seems more and more apparent that this is what has occurred (Gazzaniga, 1992).
This evolutionary perspective on brain organization and cognition suggests a number of important issues for cognitive neuroscientists (Gazzaniga, 1992). First, the implication is hard on everyday and classical ideas about learning, with the underlying assumption that any neural network can learn anything once inputs and outputs are specified. The evolutionary viewpoint would argue for not only the critical implications of the importance of specialized circuits but also the view that the human brain has a unique structural organization supporting these specialized circuits. I review data from my laboratory that is consistent with this view.
Second, the evolutionary view suggests that there should be some things humans are poor at learning. If humans have specialized circuits committed to performing particular traits, exposure to environmental stimuli to which they had not adapted might require cognitive processes they do not possess. There are many examples of such phenomena in the fields of language learning and perceptual psychology (Gazzaniga, 1992). For present purposes, these kind of data provide further evidence of the probability of identifiable capacities the human cognitive system possesses that other animals do not possess. Furthermore, because these data reflect the presence of specialized circuits, they should be found in one hemisphere or the other. There are many such examples; I relate observations from studies on the human capacity to make voluntary facial expressions (Gazzaniga & Smylie, 1990).
Specialized Circuits for Human Capacities
Accepting the view that the human brain has special circuits for carrying out its various mental functions, one can consider the various levels of organization within the nervous system where this might appear. In what follows, the argument is made that the cerebral cortex is the custodian of new circuits critical for human cognitive processes. In this light, it is commonly observed that the overall plan of the mammalian brain seems quite similar among species. This is particularly true when comparing the primate and human brain. One of the reasons comparative studies are carried out is the belief that homologous brain structures may carry out common functions in the primate and human. Yet, there are important exceptions to this overall view. The human brain, quite simply, is different from the monkey brain. There are any number of similar structures carrying out different functions in the two species. Let me review work comparing two structures that we have studied directly and indirectly in our laboratory — the anterior commissure and the superior colliculus — and relate observations that emphasize the importance of studying the human brain per se.
The animal literature clearly shows that the anterior commissure transfers visual information. Although only the callosum was found to subserve interocular transfer in cats, the anterior commissure was found to be involved in visual transfer in chimpanzees (Black & Myers, 1964) and rhesus monkeys (Gazzaniga, 1966; Sullivan & Hamilton, 1973). Taken together, this kind of evidence provides strong reasons to believe the same might be true for humans.
Prior to the development of magnetic resonance (MR) imaging, we examined some patients who, by surgical description, had their anterior commissure intact and their callosum fully sectioned. Many of these patients transferred visual information. In an effort to account for this, we proposed that the remaining anterior commissure could vary in the kinds of information it could and could not transfer. We have now had the opportunity to scan many of those supposedly split-brain patients. In fact, the MR scans show the splenium had been spared in those cases where transfer of visual information was possible, thus explaining the transfer of information. In another series of patients, similar results were noted (Gates, Leppik, Yap, & Gumnit, 1984). Every time there is evidence of transfer of visual information that requires exact matching of stimulus features following supposedly full commissurotomy, there has been sparing of the splenium. This suggests that the anterior commissure, a structure that is clearly able to transfer visual information in the monkey and chimp, does not do so in the human.
The difference seen with fiber-tract systems is also apparent in more nuclear structures such as the superior colliculus. In this case, there is clear evidence from the monkey that this structure is crucially involved in the control of eye movements. Wurtz and colleagues (Mohler & Wurtz, 1977), for example, were able to demonstrate some years ago that primates with lesions of primary visual cortex were able to detect and direct their eyes in response to visual stimuli presented in the scotoma. They suggested that the superior colliculus, working either alone or in complementary fashion with visual cortex, could carry out these functions within the scotoma. Others have claimed that even higher order functions are possible following such occipital lesions (Pasik & Pasik, 1971; Weiskrantz, Cowey, & Passingham, 1977). Although similar claims have been made for the human (Weiskrantz, 1990), we have not succeeded in demonstrating residual function following lesions to primary visual cortex (Holtzman, 1984). More recently, we have been able to carry out microperimetry of patients with occipital lesions using an image stabilizer (Fendrich, Wessinger, & Gazzaniga, 1992; Wessinger, Fendrich, & Gazzaniga, 1991).
These studies have clearly shown that patients with so-called homonomous hemianopsia can have small islands of spared vision. In these islands, there is visual function. In most of the scotoma, however, there is no visual function, confirming the earlier work of Holtzman. In short, when visual function is possible there seems to be spared visual cortex. This observation was confirmed with MR brainprinting that revealed some intact primary visual cortex. Overall, it suggests that the spared superior colliculus in the human contributes in a different way to oculomotor functions.
The clear difference in function seen between monkey and human brains, combined with possible new anatomical correlates supporting these differences, suggests a cautious note when attempting to compare the function of similar brain structures across species. It suggests the arguments against such cross-species comparisons are as crucial today as they were when originally argued years ago.
With it established that the human brain has its own unique organization, it would be interesting to see how much of this organization might be due to genetic processes. For the past few years, we have been examining this issue by studying the brains of monozygotic twins. Our initial findings regarding the corpus callosum showed that this enormous fiber-tract system was more similar in area and shape in monozygotic twins than in unrelated twins (Oppenheim, Skerry, Tramo, & Gazzaniga, 1989). Using a new method of assessing the cortical surface areas of the human brain (Jouandet et al., 1989; Loftus, 1992), we have now studied the cortical surface of both male and female monozygotic twins (Green et al., 1991; Tramo, Loftus, Newton, & Gazzaniga, 1990; Tramo, Loftus, Thomas, Green, & Gazzaniga, in prep.). Such twins look alike, talk alike, behave similarly, think similarly, and so on. Are their brains alike? Normally there is great variation in the gross morphology of the brain. Although all brains have a similar overall plan, they vary tremendously in the details. Some brains have bigger frontal lobes than others. The pattern in the visual appearance of the cortex is called the gyral/sucal pattern. It varies, and that variation presumably reflects differences in underlying brain organization. Could it be that monozygotic twins had brains that were more similar than not? The fact that their overall cognitive skills are more alike would suggest a physical basis for their more similar cognition.
Until recently, no one has had information on this crucial point. Our laboratory has been working on quantifying MR images in a way that would allow one to examine various regions in each half brain and to assess their similarity in surface area. In this technique, MR imaging is used to form some 50 image slices of the brain, which are then reconstructed to make maps of the human cerebrum. With the maps made, it is easy to measure the cortical areas of the various major lobes of the brain, allowing us to estimate surface area from the three-dimensional reconstruction of the cortical surface itself. We have discovered that 15 regions in the left and 4 regions in the right hemispheres of females showed less variance in the twins as compared to unrelated controls. For male twins, there were fewer areas in the left and about the same number in the right hemisphere. Overall, we can conclude that twin brains are more alike than unrelated brains. These data also indicate that the development of left-hemisphere structures is under considerably more genetic control for women than for men.
The Case for Unique Human System Functioning
If the human brain has unique organizational features and appears to have many of its major cortical surface areas specified by genetic mechanisms, then it might also seem likely that there would be capacities humans could engage in that other primates could not. The multitudinous new circuits in the much larger human cerebrum carry out activities other species simply do not possess. One such example of a human specialization is the capacity to make voluntary facial expressions. This is a very palpable trait of humans and easily accessible for study. It is not found in other primates including the chimpanzee (Premack, personal communication).
There is a variety of beliefs about how the brain is organized to perceive and produce facial expressions. In the perceptual domain, it appears that the right hemisphere has special processes devoted to the efficient detection of upright faces (Gazzaniga, 1989). Although the left hemisphere can also perceive and recognize faces and can reveal superior capacities when the faces are familiar, the right hemisphere appears specialized for unfamiliar facial stimuli (Gazzaniga & Smylie, 1983). Interestingly, this pattern of asymmetry for perceptual processes has also been shown for the rhesus monkey (Hamilton & Vermiere, 1988).
We recently examined the brain mechanisms involved in carrying out facial expressions in split-brain human patients. Disconnecting the two cerebral hemispheres allows the role the corpus callosum plays in controlling voluntary and involuntary expression to be assessed. It also allows examination of the ability of each hemisphere to initiate facial expressions. To understand the pattern of observed results, it is important to review the known neural mechanism active in controlling both voluntary and involuntary facial expressions.
The pattern of innervation for the upper half of the face is different from that of the lower half of the face, and the differences involve both central and peripheral systems. The neural mechanisms involved in voluntary facial postures are controlled by the cortical pyramidal system, whereas the control of spontaneous postures is managed by the extrapyramidal system. This diversity of innervation is reported to be responsible for the preservation of symmetrical spontaneous facial postures in the presence of unilateral damage to motor cortex. Patients with this lesion will evidence a contralateral facial droop that will resolve when smiling spontaneously. In this instance, althoug...

Table of contents