Introduction to Quantitative EEG and Neurofeedback
eBook - ePub

Introduction to Quantitative EEG and Neurofeedback

Advanced Theory and Applications

Thomas H. Budzynski, Helen Kogan Budzynski, James R. Evans, Andrew Abarbanel, James R. Evans, Thomas H. Budzynski, Helen Kogan Budzynski, Andrew Abarbanel

Share book
  1. 528 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Introduction to Quantitative EEG and Neurofeedback

Advanced Theory and Applications

Thomas H. Budzynski, Helen Kogan Budzynski, James R. Evans, Andrew Abarbanel, James R. Evans, Thomas H. Budzynski, Helen Kogan Budzynski, Andrew Abarbanel

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

The study of Quantitative EEGs and Neurofeedback offer a window into brain physiology and function via computer and statistical analyses, suggesting innovative approaches to the improvement of attention, anxiety, mood and behavior. Resources for understanding what QEEG and Neurofeedback is, how they are used, and to what disorders and patients they can be applied are scarce, and this volume serves as an ideal tool for clinical researchers and practicing clinicians, providing a broad overview of the most interesting topics relating to the techniques. The revised coverage of advancements, new applications (e.g. Aspberger's, music therapy, LORETA, etc.), and combinations of prior approaches make the second edition a necessary companion to the first. The top scholars in the field have been enlisted and contributions will offer both the breadth needed for an introductory scholar and the depth desired by a clinical professional.

  • Detailed new protocols for treatment of anxiety, depression, ADHD, and PTSD
  • Newest protocol in Z-score training enables clinicians to extend their practices
  • LORETA diagnostic tool lets the clinician watch for changes deep in the brain through working with surface EEG patterns

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Introduction to Quantitative EEG and Neurofeedback an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Introduction to Quantitative EEG and Neurofeedback by Thomas H. Budzynski, Helen Kogan Budzynski, James R. Evans, Andrew Abarbanel, James R. Evans, Thomas H. Budzynski, Helen Kogan Budzynski, Andrew Abarbanel in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Psychology & Clinical Psychology. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2009
ISBN
9780080923154
Edition
2
Part I
QEEG and Neurofeedback: Basics and New Theory
Outline
Chapter 1

Neuromodulation technologies

An attempt at classification

Siegfried Othmer, Ph.D., EEG Institute, Woodland Hills, California, USA

Publisher Summary

This chapter addresses the question of how to classify the neuromodulation effects resulting from widely differing neurofeedback approaches developed over the last four decades. A proliferation of targets and objectives has been observed to which attention is directed in the training. With regard to clinical outcomes, however, one encounters a broad zone of commonality. Why is it that the premises and technological approaches within the neurofeedback network of scholars and clinicians are so disparate, yet they largely achieve common clinical goals? This in-depth analysis may lead one closer to the “essence” of neurofeedback and provide focus for further development efforts. This chapter attempts to appraise the “state of the field” at this moment. The objective is to discern the commonalities among the various approaches on the one hand, and among the clinical findings on the other. This will lead to a codification of a “minimal set of claims” that could serve to cover the commonalities among the techniques, and it will lead to a simple classification scheme for the various clinical findings. The evidence in favor of such a minimal set of claims will be adduced largely by reference.

I Introduction

This chapter addresses the question of how to classify the neuromodulation effects resulting from widely differing neurofeedback approaches developed over the last four decades. We have seen a proliferation of targets and objectives to which attention is directed in the training. With regard to clinical outcomes, however, one encounters a broad zone of commonality. Why is it that the premises and technological approaches within the neurofeedback network of scholars and clinicians are so disparate, yet they largely achieve common clinical goals? This in-depth analysis may lead us closer to the “essence” of neurofeedback and provide focus for further development efforts.
In its most common applications, EEG feedback typically combines two challenges—one directed to the frequency-based organization of brain communication and one that targets inappropriate state transitions. These two challenges lead to very different rules of engagement. As such rules are unearthed, they must be understood in terms of an appropriate model of brain function. At a more philosophical level, an understanding of this whole process also takes us to the very cusp of the mind–body problem, the neural network relations that provide the nexus where our thoughts are encoded and interact directly and inseparably with network representations of psychophysiological states.
This chapter will attempt to appraise the “state of the field” at this moment. The objective is to discern the commonalities among the various approaches on the one hand, and among the clinical findings on the other. This will lead to a codification of a “minimal set of claims” that could serve to cover the commonalities among the techniques, and it will lead to a simple classification scheme for the various clinical findings. The evidence in favor of such a minimal set of claims will be adduced largely by reference. Further, the classification of the various clinical findings will serve the objective of a more appropriate or natural language for the field of neurotherapy than is provided in the formalism of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV, APA, 1994).

II Tracing the historical threads of neurofeedback

A. The alpha rhythm and “felt states”

The field of neurofeedback began in two threads of research that concerned themselves with one or another of the primary resting rhythms of the EEG. Here the local synchrony of the neuronal assemblies was such that the EEG amplitude would episodically rise above the ambient into dominant spindle-burst activity. In the case of the alpha rhythm, the feature was so obvious in the record that it became the first identified signature of the EEG in the original discovery of Hans Berger (1929). Joe Kamiya then first studied it in relation to our felt states, the question addressed being whether the human subject is able to have any kind of awareness regarding his own alpha activity (Kamiya, 1968). An affirmative finding eventually led to active reinforcement on alpha spindle incidence getting underway (Hardt and Kamiya, 1976). The preoccupation with subjective states of awareness and of feeling, however, was not consonant with the prevailing Zeitgeist, and Kamiya’s research found little resonance in the broader field of psychology.

B. The sensorimotor rhythm and behavioral state

The work of Maurice Barry Sterman very consciously took a different tack. First of all, the work utilized animal subjects, so there was no question of inquiring into felt states, but that would not have been Sterman’s inclination in any event. The thrust was to connect the realm of the EEG with that of overt behavior and of behavioral states. The focus became the sensorimotor rhythm, spindle-burst EEG activity localized to the cat sensorimotor cortex that was observable even in the waking state during periods of motoric stillness. It was observed that training the behavior in order to manifest the SMR spindle was not as efficient as rewarding the animal for the appearance of the SMR spindle and having behavioral stillness as the concomitant. Either way, however, the phenomena were coupled (Sterman et al., 1969). (For a review of this early research that ties into later neurofeedback, see Othmer et al., 1999.)
When attention later turned to the use of this simple reinforcement technique for the suppression of seizure susceptibility in humans, the training had to be done under circumstances in which the EEG was often not well-behaved as it had been in the cats. Sterman was the first to install inhibit functions on this account, but the intent was simply to assure that inappropriate triggers of a reward were suppressed. Significantly, the focus of the work remained entirely on the elicitation of an SMR bursting response. A second issue was that the human waking EEG did not manifest SMR spindles that clearly rose above the background activity, as was the case for cats. But the training philosophy carried over, as only extreme SMR amplitude excursions were rewarded. This was expected to either end up in skewing the SMR amplitude distribution or perhaps in moving the entire distribution to higher amplitudes. The focus on seizure management placed this method within the domain of neurology, but it was unlikely then (and remains unlikely now) that the field of neurology would look favorably upon behavioral interventions. (For a review, see Sterman, 2000.)

C. EEG operant feedback and normalcy patterns

Joel Lubar was the first to employ inhibit functions with the overt objective of training toward more normal distributions. This proscriptive aspect of the training imposed its own rules on the training task, and also made for a non-prescriptive appeal to the brain that differed considerably from what was involved in the reward-based training. It also elevated the issue of EEG normalcy as a guiding principle to EEG reinforcement protocols, with profound implications for the emerging field. (For a review of this early work, see Nash, 2000.)

D. Stimulation-based treatment

Paralleling the above developments were various stimulation-based approaches to brain-state alteration, mainly using audio-visual modes. Indeed this work had its earliest precursors in the work of Adrian and Matthews (1934), who evaluated optical stimulation in their replication of Berger’s rhythm. It therefore preceded EEG feedback by some three decades. But audio-visual stimulation had suffered the same fate as Kamiya’s work of being taken up by a variety of enthusiasts over the years, which then spoiled it for the attentions of academic researchers. Stimulation-based techniques have since come to be seen as competitive with reward-based feedback in terms of clinical efficacy, and must therefore be included in any comprehensive appraisal of the field. The evidence for this is strongest for ADHD (Siever, 2007). In order to accommodate both neurofeedback and stimulation the more inclusive term of neuromodulation will be used below.
The development of the field subsequent to the early initiatives by Kamiya, Sterman, and Lubar has been modestly evolutionary, but the essential character of the work was laid down during the early days of the field, and threads of continuity carry through to this day. Only the stimulation-based work requires a separate treatment. The subsequent discussion is conducted more at the conceptual level rather than being constructed strictly upon the established empirical basis. Of course empirical data drive the discussion, but it would be premature to make fine distinctions on the basis of the available evidence, or to be too judgmental at this point, for example with respect to the relative efficacy of the various techniques.
Most if not all of the approaches remain in a state of both technical and tactical immaturity. Moreover, the clinical world is not restricted to using only one mode but will likely see the emergence of a multiplicity of techniques and combinations of techniques for synergistic effects. The question of which is best therefore does not even merit a response at this time. And by the time the question can be answered well, it will hopefully no longer be relevant.

III A classification of neuromodulation technologies

At the top level we may partition the field into volitional and non-volitional approaches, with feedback techniques generally falling into the former category and stimulation-based techniques into the latter. This is in line with the traditional focus on “voluntary controls” in the biofeedback literature, and with the emphasis on recruitment of the individual’s efforts and intentions in the service of better self-regulation in traditional biofeedback. Clinical experience w...

Table of contents