Instrumental Music Education
eBook - ePub

Instrumental Music Education

Teaching with the Musical and Practical in Harmony

Evan Feldman, Ari Contzius

Share book
  1. 480 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Instrumental Music Education

Teaching with the Musical and Practical in Harmony

Evan Feldman, Ari Contzius

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Instrumental Music Education: Teaching with the Musical and Practical in Harmony, Third Edition, is intended for college instrumental music education majors studying to be band and orchestra directors at the elementary, middle school, and high school levels. This textbook presents a research-based look at the topics vital to running a successful instrumental music program, while balancing musical, theoretical, and practical approaches. A central theme is the compelling parallel between language and music, including "sound-to-symbol" pedagogies. Understanding this connection improves the teaching of melody, rhythm, composition, and improvisation.

The companion website contains over 120 pedagogy videos for wind, string, and percussion instru­ments performed by professional players and teachers, over 50 rehearsal videos, rhythm flashcards, and two additional chapters: "The Rehearsal Toolkit" and ''Job Search and Interview." It also includes over 50 tracks of acoustically pure drones and demonstration exercises for use in rehearsals, sectionals, and lessons.

New to This Edition:

  • A new chapter on teaching beginning band using sound-to-symbol pedagogies
  • Expanded coverage for strings and orchestra, including a new chapter on teaching beginning strings
  • A new chapter on conducting technique
  • Expanded material on teaching students with disabilities
  • Concert etiquette and the concert experience
  • Expanded coverage on the science of learning, including the Dunning-Kruger effect and the effective use of repetition in rehearsal
  • Techniques for improving students' practice habits

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Instrumental Music Education an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Instrumental Music Education by Evan Feldman, Ari Contzius in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Mezzi di comunicazione e arti performative & Musica. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2020
ISBN
9780429647536
Unit 1
The Process of Teaching Instrumental Music

Chapter 1

An Introduction to Sound–to–Symbol

To Guide Your Reading

What is “sound–to–symbol” and how does it differ from other teaching approaches used in the music classroom?
What connections are there between music and language?
Why are these connections important when considering an approach to teaching music?
How does treating music as a language affect the way we teach it?
What are the fundamental principles of Suzuki, KodĂĄly, Orff, and Gordon?
In what ways can competing sound–to–symbol theories co-exist and support each other in the classroom?
What are the common questions, confusions, and misconceptions people have concerning these teaching approaches?
How can sound–to–symbol principles be integrated with non-sound–to–symbol curriculums and method texts?
Consider the structure of many beginning instrumental music programs: After students choose their instrument, the first lessons cover how to hold it, how to shape an embouchure, where to place the fingers, how to produce an initial tone, how to read simple rhythms, how to hold a bow, and how to apply correct fingerings. Students are shown a whole note, told it contains four quarter notes and gets four beats, and are then asked to play that note, usually on the easiest tone to produce. More rhythms and notes are introduced until students are prepared for simple melodies, which they read using their newly learned knowledge about notation and fingerings.
Conservatories and professional orchestras are filled with exceptional musicians who are products of this system. Nevertheless, traditional method books may not reflect the way people naturally learn music, which many educational theorists argue parallels language learning. This is a process that begins with learning to speak and only then moves on to learning to read. This is often called “sound–to–symbol,” or “sound before sign,” because it emphasizes aural recognition and understanding as precursors to theoretical knowledge and reading skills.
As we will see in Chapter 5, sound–to–symbol is not a new concept. Johann Heinrich Pestalozzi (1746–1827), the Swiss educational reformer, Hans Georg Nägeli (1773–1836), Pestalozzi’s disciple, Sarah Ann Glover (1785–1867), the first developer of the sol-fa system, and John Curwen (1816–1880), the English minister who refined it, all advocated similar philosophies. Yet somehow, at least in American public school instrumental music education, it remains on the periphery.
In the twentieth and twenty-first centuries, at least five music education philosophies reflect the sound–to–symbol approach: Kodály, Suzuki, Orff, Gordon’s Music Learning Theory, and Dalcroze Eurhythmics (which will be discussed in detail in Chapter 2). These music philosophies rely on three basic assumptions, all of which are connected in some way to spoken language:
1.Nearly everybody acquires the ability to speak without the benefit of formal training.
2.The processes of acquiring language and music parallel each other in key ways.
3.One of the most important steps in learning language or music is experiencing it and doing it.

The Connection between Language and Music

Despite Henry Wadsworth Longfellow’s observation that it is the universal language, music is not technically classified as one. But perhaps it does not have to be a language in order to act like one. This is why sound–to–symbol philosophies assume a strong connection between language and music. This is common sense to many musicians, and the fields of linguistics, cognitive evolutionary science, and neuroscience are finding increasing evidence to support the connection.
Famed linguist Noam Chomsky was inspired by children’s innate ability to learn complex linguistic skills on their own, a process he called “linguistic competence.”1 Marveling over their ability to produce grammatically correct sentences they had never before heard, he postulated that all children are born with “formal universals”—genetically encoded rules that operate in all languages.2
Leonard Bernstein seized upon this idea and makes a case for a musical version of it in his 1973 lecture series at Harvard, The Unanswered Question. In searching for a musical grammar that explains how we innately hear music, Bernstein painstakingly applies Chomsky’s grammatical rules to musical analogues, a process he admits is only “quasi-scientific.”3

Bernstein’s Musical–Linguistic Analogues

musical motive ⇨ noun
chord or harmony ⇨ adjective
rhythm ⇨ verb
Though they break down if we dig too deeply, Bernstein’s analogies are food for thought as we work to establish a meaningful language–music connection. Fortunately, cognitive evolutionary research and brain research pick up the argument where poetic reasoning leaves off. Although the brain’s music and language systems seem to operate independently, they share neurobiological origins in the same regions.4 This helps explain how music shares several features with speech. Among them:
1.A set of grammatical and syntactical rules that govern a hierarchy of how structures (words, notes, etc.) are ordered and arranged.
2.Memorized “representations”—In the case of language, these representations are words and their meanings; in the case of music, these representations are melodies.
Music and language share important brain processes that interpret rhythmic structure.5 For example, both music and speech group sound into patterns called phrases. There is also evidence of overlap in how the brain processes the boundaries between phrases.6
A study published in the Journal of Neuroscience, by Dr. Nina Kraus, showed not only that rhythm is related to language, but also that rhythm skills developed through music lead to strong language skills. Dr. Kraus measured how well a group of 100 teenagers were able to tap their fingers to a beat. She found that musically trained subjects who were best able to tap the pulse also showed enhanced neural response to language. According to Kraus, “It turns out that kids who are poor readers have a lot of difficulty doing this motor task and following the beat. In both speech and music, rhythm provides a temporal map with signposts to the most likely locations of meaningful input.”7
As described by Aniruddh D. Patel in his book, Music, Language, and the Brain, the brain also seems to process melodic contours in music and language in a similar way.8 If we examine the makeup of those contours closely, we find that both music and language share a predominance of small intervals between pitches,9 something that could be explained by the physiological limitations of the human voice. But studies by Patel, Iversen, and Rosenberg (2006) reveal characteristics in the music of countries that reflect the tonal patterns of those countries’ languages.10 The study found that English speech patterns appear in English music and French speech patterns appear in French music11—and these are differences that...

Table of contents