The State of the American Mind
eBook - ePub

The State of the American Mind

16 Leading Critics on the New Anti-Intellectualism

Mark Bauerlein, Adam Bellow, Mark Bauerlein, Adam Bellow

Share book
  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

The State of the American Mind

16 Leading Critics on the New Anti-Intellectualism

Mark Bauerlein, Adam Bellow, Mark Bauerlein, Adam Bellow

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

In 1987, Allan Bloom's The Closing of the American Mind was published; a wildly popular book that drew attention to the shift in American culture away from the tenants that made America—and Americans—unique. Bloom focused on a breakdown in the American curriculum, but many sensed that the issue affected more than education. The very essence of what it meant to be an American was disappearing.That was over twenty years ago. Since then, the United States has experienced unprecedented wealth, more youth enrolling in higher education than ever before, and technology advancements far beyond what many in the 1980s dreamed possible. And yet, the state of the American mind seems to have deteriorated further. Benjamin Franklin's "self-made man" has become a man dependent on the state. Independence has turned into self-absorption. Liberty has been curtailed in the defense of multiculturalism.In order to fully grasp the underpinnings of this shift away from the self-reliant, well-informed American, editors Mark Bauerlein and Adam Bellow have brought together a group of cultural and educational experts to discuss the root causes of the decline of the American mind. The writers of these fifteen original essays include E. D. Hirsch, Nicholas Eberstadt, and Dennis Prager, as well as Daniel Dreisbach, Gerald Graff, Richard Arum, Robert Whitaker, David T. Z. Mindich, Maggie Jackson, Jean Twenge, Jonathan Kay, Ilya Somin, Steve Wasserman, Greg Lukianoff, and R. R. Reno. Their essays are compiled into three main categories:

  • States of Mind: Indicators of Intellectual and Cognitive Decline
    • These essays broach specific mental deficiencies among the population, including lagging cultural IQ, low Biblical literacy, poor writing skills, and over-medication.
  • Personal and Cognitive Habits/Interests
    • These essays turn to specific mental behaviors and interests, including avoidance of the news, short attention spans, narcissism, and conspiracy obsessions.
  • National Consequences
    • These essays examine broader trends affecting populations and institutions, including rates of entitlement claims, voting habits, and a low-performing higher education system.

The State of the American Mind is both an assessment of our current state as well as a warning, foretelling what we may yet become. For anyone interested in the intellectual fate of America, The State of the American Mind offers an accessible and critical look at life in America and how our collective mind is faring.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is The State of the American Mind an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access The State of the American Mind by Mark Bauerlein, Adam Bellow, Mark Bauerlein, Adam Bellow in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Politics & International Relations & Politics. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.
Part One
image
States of Mind
Indicators of Intellectual and Cognitive Decline
image
1
The Troubling Trend of Cultural IQ
Mark Bauerlein
ANY BROAD DISCUSSION of the American Mind has to take into account an extraordinary finding first noticed in the middle of the twentieth century and confirmed and publicized widely in the 1980s and beyond. From World War I to World War II, cognitive psychologists discovered, the average score of U.S. soldiers on mental tests rose significantly, and in the decades after that, scores on IQ tests continued the trend. It came to be known as the “Flynn Effect,” named for New Zealand social scientist James R. Flynn, whose studies brought to widespread attention the intelligence gains by adults and children in the United States and elsewhere.
IQ tests always set the mean at 100, adjusting the scale accordingly over time so that it appears that test takers from year to year achieve the same cumulative score (a 100 places one at the fiftieth percentile). But if we were to maintain the same scale over the decades, without recalibrating it to keep the mean the same, we would find it rising, as follows for the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC):
100.00 (1947–48) 107.63 (1972) 113.00 (1989) 117.63 (2002)
And here are the scores for the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS):
100.00 (1953–54) 107.50 (1978) 111.70 (1995) 115.07 (2006)
An average child in 2002, that is, would have scored seventeen points above average in the late 1940s, while an average adult in 2006 would beat the average in the early 1950s by fifteen points. In other words, an average person in 2002 (the fiftieth percentile) would fall into the eighty-fifth percentile in the 1947–48 group. This advance is remarkable, one that few scientists would have thought possible. As Flynn’s studies circulated in the 1980s and after, psychologists analyzed the data and pondered their causes and implications at length. What made American intelligence climb so rapidly, and will the gains continue? Do the numbers indicate that Americans are, in fact, getting smarter—more knowledgeable, critical, ingenious, and reflective?
Explanations for the rising scores have varied, with researchers advancing several factors in different proportions, including improvements in early childhood nutrition and education, more people going to college, reduced exposure to toxins (particularly lead), a more demanding cognitive environment in the workplace and in leisure activities (created by technology and media), and the expansion of practices of hypothesizing and abstract reasoning. Flynn favors the latter cause, noting that IQ test questions mostly ask people to detach facts from concrete situations and apply logic to them, identifying patterns, similarities, and analogies, not invoking real-world knowledge. They require subjects to perform certain mental operations—for instance, examining numbers in sequence and predicting the next one to come, or observing objects in two pictures and determining relationships between them, then looking at a third picture and selecting a fourth picture (out of five provided) that forms the same relationship to it. Knowledge helps, but mainly knowledge that supports abstract inferences (such as familiarity with classification schemes and mathematical processes), not factual knowledge such as what happened on December 7, 1941. The actual objects in the pictures themselves don’t matter, only the abstract relations one can draw. Consider the question, “What do dogs and rabbits have in common?” A concrete answer might be, “Well, dogs help you hunt rabbits,” while an abstract one would go, “Both are mammals.” As Flynn notes in discussing this example in Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century (2012), IQ exams such as Raven’s Progressive Matrices test for the abstract choice. He terms it a “habit of mind,” a readiness to treat objects in a conditional, scientific way. “If you are unaccustomed to using logic for anything but to deal with the concrete world,” he writes, “and indeed distrust reasoning that is not grounded in the concrete, you are not amenable to the change of gears that Raven’s requires” (13). If a test question began, “Suppose two friends start from Point A and reach Point B five minutes apart . . . ,” a hyperconcrete mind might think, “Why don’t they drive the same speed?” while an abstract-habit mind would comfortably accept the opening as a working premise. Rising IQ scores, then, measure a particular kind of cognitive disposition that, in fact, primary and secondary education in the twentieth century had increasingly stressed. The gains are real, and they mean we are getting smarter—if, that is, we define intelligence as this capacity for abstraction.
Indeed, if we take the numbers at face value, we have to believe that American adults and children are much smarter than they were a half century ago, as if evolution accelerated so fast that it concentrated thousands of years of development into several dozen. The average child in 2010 would have been exceptional in 1950, the ordinary twenty-first-century adult able to run circles around a mid-twentieth-century adult whenever an abstract intellectual problem arose. That’s what fifteen points represent—clear and distinct superiority, a marked gap in mental talent easy to demonstrate (hand both of them a Sudoku puzzle). A score of 117 puts someone in the top 15 percent of the population.
And yet, given this dramatic advance, those of us in the worlds of education and employment are stuck with a discordant fact. Why, we wonder, do so many high school students, college students, and younger workers seem so terribly deficient in basic knowledge and skills? Their reasoning abilities may have jumped forward, their ability to learn may make the 1950s mind appear lethargic, but their reading comprehension hasn’t improved at all. One source of long-term data on reading comes from the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP—the “Nation’s Report Card”), which shows scores for seventeen-year-olds flat since the test began in the early 1970s (National Center for Education Statistics). We have average scores on the SAT verbal exam going back even farther, and they underwent a shocking decline of fifty-four points from 1962 to 1980. The score has wavered up and down by a few points ever since (Adams). More youths are going to college, yes, but the numbers of them requiring remedial coursework in math and writing keep climbing, the rate of those in need standing at 10 percent for selective schools, 30 percent at less selective colleges, and 60 percent at two-year institutions, according to the National Center for Public Policy and Higher Education. Employers, too, constantly grumble about the lack of readiness of college graduates for their workplaces. A fall 2013 report by Chegg queried one thousand hiring managers on the skills of recent college graduates, and the rate of those who judged them “Very/Completely Prepared” for the following tasks came up as follows:
image
“Working on tasks independently”: 58 percent
image
“Writing to sum up results”: 51 percent
image
“Solving problems through experimentation”: 49 percent
image
“Making a persuasive argument”: 43 percent
At the same time, Chegg asked college students the same question, and they judged themselves “Very/Completely Prepared” at much higher rates, for instance, 70 percent on “Writing to sum up results” (Korn). More substandard results come from a 2013 ACT report on the entire adult population, not just recent graduates, that identifies three “cognitive assessments” (“Reading for Information,” “Applied Mathematics,” and “Locating Information”) that tally more closely the mental aptitudes tested by IQ questions. “Less than half (45%) of the examinees with a high level of education met the Locating Information skill requirements for 4 of the 5 occupations with a large number of openings and for 3 of the 5 highest-paying occupations requiring a high level of education,” the ACT concluded. “Only 19% of the examinees with a low level of education met or exceeded the Locating Information skill requirement.” Overall, “Significant foundational skills gaps exist between the skills of examinees with either a low or high level of education and the skills needed for jobs requiring a low or high level of education.” Clearly, the gains reported on IQ tests haven’t spread to school and work.
If we drill down into the IQ data, however, the mismatch starts to make sense. The WISC (child) and WAIS (adult) IQ tests have several parts, each one testing a different mental function. The Digit Span subtest has subjects repeat a list of numbers that have been presented orally (testing memory and attention). The Block Design on the children’s test asks subjects to organize blocks according to a given model (testing spatial reasoning and motor skills), while the Vocabulary exam for adults tests how many words they know and how well they can deploy them. Other subtests include Visual Puzzles, Similarities, Arithmetic, Coding, Matrix Reasoning, Symbol Search, and Information (a straightforward measure of general knowledge derived from culture). If we look at child scores, we see that the rates of improvement over the decades have varied for each subtest, sometimes sharply, the difference masked when we combine them into an average score. At the high end, we find Similarities climbing 23.85 points from 1950 to 2004, while Coding jumped 18 points and Block Design 15.9 points. At the low end, the Information sub-test showed an improvement of only 2.15 points, while Arithmetic inched up only 2.3 points and Vocabulary results added 4.4 points. The lesser subtest outcomes explain why academics have stalled for U.S. schoolchildren. Flynn sets NAEP reading scores alongside Information and Vocabulary data for the years 1972 to 2002, when general-information knowledge was flat and vocabulary moved minimally. On the NAEP reading exams, elementary and middle-school students showed some improvements, but twelfth-graders none at all, leading Flynn to conclude, “Today’s children may learn to master basic reading skills at a younger age, but are no better prepared for reading more demanding adult literature” (18). The small vocabulary IQ gain is reflected in better NAEP reading scores by nine-year-olds, but it doesn’t help seventeen-year-olds whose reading tests contain passages with diction exceeding that gain.
The Information subtest factors into reading scores as well, a connection that laypersons, educators, and even professors of education often fail to appreciate. The significance of general information begins with the fact that texts always have unstated meanings—content that is implicit in the sentences and taken for granted as known by readers. The 2009 NAEP reading assessment for twelfth-graders, for instance, chose as one passage a rental agreement and asked test takers to respond to prompts such as, “The name of the tenant must be filled in on the rental agreement in two places. Identify the two places and explain why the name of the tenant needs to appear in each of them.” The passage didn’t explain what a rental agreement is, define key terms (“tenant,” “disclosure”), or introduce the diction and style of legal language. Successful test takers had to know something about property rights and contracts in order to read quickly and answer the questions confidently. Those who didn’t stumbled as they read, laboring to discern meanings that others apprehended automatically. Examine any newspaper story, press release, scientific report, contemporary novel, or other commonplace text and you find the same thing: buried knowledge that sets readers on a comprehension continuum, those at the high end having the knowledge that lets them proceed fluently, and those at the low end not having it and faltering. If you don’t know anything about rental situations and have never seen a legal contract, critical thinking skills won’t much help. Those abstract mental abilities measured by other subtests, at best, help readers to make imperfect inferences from whatever aspects of the text are familiar. But that’s a time-consuming and chancy process. Relevant prior knowledge, in contrast, allows readers to recognize implicit references and situations in a snap. The contrast has become clear in eye-tracking studies. Readers with some knowledge about the topic of the text move their eyes swiftly and linearly through the text, whereas low-knowledge readers’ eyes move haltingly, doubling back and slowing down. Their ensuing scores reflect their ease or difficulty.
The more background knowledge one has, then, the more agile and capacious a reader can be, which is why the Information and Vocabulary IQ trends are so important to the state of the American Mind. Other IQ subtest scores give ample reason for optimism, but the disappointing stagnation of general knowledge and vocabulary among the young in the last sixty years means that the benefits of the Flynn Effect may be limited to bounded spheres in which general knowledge and word breadth don’t count so much, such as undemanding entertainments and unskilled labor, but not in the civic sphere, traditional cultural settings such as performance halls, or any place related to a marketplace of ideas, not to mention professional and managerial workplaces. Better spatial reasoning may help people read a map and navigate a video game, but it won’t help them choose better schools for their children, catch biblical references in President Obama’s speeches, decide which mayoral candidate to vote for, or evaluate the trustworthiness of various news programs.
Adults have produced steady IQ gains on knowledge-driven sub-tests, rising 17.8 points in Vocabulary and a not-so-impressive 8.4 points in Information since 1950, the latter likely a result of many more people going to college and picking up general knowledge by enrolling in basic courses in U.S. history, economics, psychology, English, and other core subjects. (The virtually flat Information scores for youths indicate that the boost must happen after age seventeen.) Given the explosion of college attendance in the second half of the twentieth century, not to mention the proliferation of information about the world through the advent of television, we have to judge the adult improvement in Information a disappointment. Set alongside Vocabulary, Coding (16.15 points—a typical coding task is to copy a series of numbers and symbols in sequence within a time limit), and Similarities (19.55 points), Information looks like less of a positive development than it does a result casting doubt upon the capacity of abstract cognitive skills to incorporate concrete facts about history, politics, arts, and culture and make people curious about them. In contrast to Arithmetic, though, where adults gained only 3.5 points, Information looks gigantic (Flynn judges the minimal adult gain on Arithmetic “unexpected and shocking” [22]). Still, if adults and children raised their abstract, fluid intelligence so much, why couldn’t they have raised their general knowledge at parallel rates? What would have happened in college if students had entered with a significant knowledge gain already under way?
We can sharpen the question by focusing on Vocabulary and identifying an empirical puzzle. As adults have raised their general knowledge IQ by 8 points, presumably they have retained the knowledge long after they left college, got married, and had children. Greater knowledge certainly affects interests and conversation in the home, and we would expect a more knowledge-rich environment to have influenced the children, producing a parallel rise. But that hasn’t happened, as the 6-point difference between the two groups indicates. As for Vocabulary, it should have shown an even firmer correlation between adult and child IQ, because vocabulary learning for small children transpires mainly through the environment that adults provide, including their conversation and reading material. Here, however, we find the most conspicuous gap of all between older and younger Americans. As noted above, from 1950 to 2004, WISC scores for Vocabulary rose 4.4 points, while WA...

Table of contents