Spelling for Life
eBook - ePub

Spelling for Life

Uncovering the Simplicity and Science of Spelling

Lyn Stone

Share book
  1. 208 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Spelling for Life

Uncovering the Simplicity and Science of Spelling

Lyn Stone

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

There is a myth that English spelling is unnecessarily complex, and it is spread by those who don't understand the writing system. Spelling for Life offers lucid, accessible tools which help to reveal that, when explicitly and systematically taught, spelling is scientific, law-abiding and even elegant.

Using a synthesis of theory, research and teaching experience, the fascinating nature of English spelling is systematically teased out. The examples and exercises throughout offer an encouraging, accessible way to implement the program of study and strive to reveal the beauty of spelling. Spelling for Life enables teachers and students to:

• learn what the common spelling coping strategies are;

• gain insights into undoing poor spelling habits;

• work together to reveal patterns not only in regular spelling, but also in words which on the surface seem to break the spelling rules;

• practise successful spelling strategies, progressing from simple to complex words rapidly and with confidence.

This new and improved edition includes updated spelling techniques as well as new chapters on orthographic mapping, spelling assessment, teaching consonant clusters well and suffixing rules. Aided by example lessons, formative assessments, unique tools, a scope and sequence, and extensive practice lists, this highly acclaimed overview of spelling succeeds in developing theory and practice in the writing system for teacher and student alike.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Spelling for Life an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Spelling for Life by Lyn Stone in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Education & Education General. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Routledge
Year
2021
ISBN
9781000363869
Edition
2

1 Orthographic mapping

How do children become fluent readers and writers? They do so by building up a store of words they can recognise effortlessly, without sounding them out and without looking elsewhere for cues. In fact, once they have a word in their sight word vocabulary, they cannot suppress its sound and possible meanings when they come to that word again.
This is the most useful meaning of the phrase sight word. There are some who define sight words as ‘words that do not follow simple phoneme–grapheme correspondence rules’. Words like ‘eye’ and ‘could’, ‘would’, ‘should’, etc. might fall into this category. However, this type of word is better described as irregular.
Irregular words are only classified as irregular until their structure has been analyzed and explained. All words are spelled the way they are for a reason. Unfamiliar spellings only seem irregular.
Another common but inaccurate definition of sight words is ‘words which need to be learned by looking at the whole word and memorizing it as a whole’. This look-say method of teaching reading grew like a noxious weed in education in the last century. It was based on the erroneous assumption that storage and retrieval of words is an act of visual memory. It is not. Our eyes are the first processors when encountering words, that is true, but research has shown that word storage and retrieval are not functions of the visual system (Kilpatrick 2016). A blind person would not be able to learn to read if vision were the key driver in word learning.
In actual fact, it is the hearing-impaired population that gets the raw deal when learning to read, since so much of the early foundations for literacy are laid using the auditory, not visual, system.
Irregular or not, how are words memorised in the first place? Think about the massive amount of words and word parts you recognise instantly. Did you consciously memorise every single word you ever encountered? Let’s place it into perspective: The average literate adult can automatically recognise between 30,000 and 70,000 words and word parts (Kilpatrick 2016). Did we do some kind of memorisation trick 70,000 times? Did someone teach us every single one? No, that would be impossible.
What we did was use orthographic mapping and self-teaching to gain our permanent sight word vocabulary.
Linnea Ehri, a researcher at the University of California, spent much of her time focusing on this process and coined the term orthographic mapping (Ehri 2014).
When learning to read, children need to be taught to look at the sequence of letters on the page, translate them into possible phonemes that they represent, and then blend those phonemes to form pronunciations of whole words.
So they map the symbols, to the sounds, to the words. The letter represents the sound, which, in conjunction with the other letters and sounds, represents the word. These sequences then become bonded, eventually resulting in effortless decoding, regardless of how complex the pattern is. So a word like straight can be stored just as efficiently as cat, if the person has highly developed orthographic mapping skills (Figure 1.1).
Figure 1.1 Orthographic mapping diagram

Ehri’s key findings

  • Children who have good knowledge of grapheme–phoneme correspondences can retain words in their long-term memory with more efficiency than those who don’t.
  • Underpinning this proficiency is phonemic awareness.
  • The crucial part of this kind of memorisation is attention to the sequence of letters in a word.
  • This builds cumulatively and eventually results in seemingly effortless decoding of familiar and, more importantly, unfamiliar or irregular words.
This is where Share’s Self-Teaching Hypothesis comes in.

Share’s Self-Teaching Hypothesis

There comes a point in a student’s life that I call the sweet spot. It’s where, after sometimes many hours, weeks, or months of seemingly endless effort, they begin to read unfamiliar words without them being explicitly taught.
According to psychologist and researcher David L. Share, students with sufficient phonological awareness and phonics knowledge begin to apply those skills to novel words and start to independently bond those sequences into long-term memory. This is known as phonological recoding (Share 1995).
The important point is that again, the process involves phonological recoding. It cannot take place without attention to phonology, conscious or not.
It stands to reason, then, that acquisition of novel words, and eventually of literacy overall, relies on taking that phonological route. Guessing, skipping, looking at pictures, or memorising whole words on flashcards interferes with this process. It is especially disastrous for spelling.

Some ideas for teaching to enhance orthographic mapping

Think carefully about your grouping technique

Group words to be learned as a spelling focus into close, logical families.
This can be done along orthographic, etymological, or morphological lines (and those lines often overlap). Be careful though, when attempting to group words along phonological lines. Grouping them by sound alone is inefficient, given that there can be multiple spellings for one sound.
Words are spelled the way they are because of where they came from and which families they belong to. Grouping by sound essentially ignores all of that rich, generalizable information.
In terms of cognitive load, lists containing multiple spellings place far too much pressure on working memory. A successful system for learning spelling is one which pays attention to cognitive load and pace.
Figure 1.2 is an example of a word list, grouped by sound, that violates the principles of cognitive load, explicit instruction, and a systematic approach.
Figure 1.2 An example of very low-quality spelling instruction
Trying to teach spelling this way is like trying to teach biology by saying, ‘Let’s memorise all the things with legs’.
The unfortunate child who was given the list in Figure 1.2, to cram for a Friday test, was asked to memorise words in the first section. The children in his class who weren’t particularly struggling with spelling were asked to memorise the middle words. The words in the last section were for the good spellers only. No definitions, no structure, no morphology, nothing was offered in the way of study of these words. And the teacher would happily say she was ‘doing phonics’, not because she was a bad teacher, but because her teaching degree placed no importance on the structure of language and failed to come into the 21st century in terms of reading science.
Close families, related by more than just their sounds, will win every time.

Don’t discount copying

Develop your students’ ability to copy efficiently. Copying words, sentences, and paragraphs is not only a great way to practise fluency and spelling, using a scaffolded, stable framework, but also, if used purposefully, can enhance everything else that constitutes writing.

Establish productive practice routines

Drill new words, first by sounding each phoneme whilst following the sequence of the letters, and then by saying the whole word.
Please don’t ask students to write the words over and over again. Anyone who has ever had the punishment of writing lines knows that after a few repetitions, those words become scratches on a page and lose all their meaning.
There is also the distinct possibility of error reinforcement if the original word was transcribed wrongly.
To map words efficiently, students have to attend to their pronunciation in combination with the sequence of letters in the word.
Interleaving the target words throughout your teaching sequence also leads to efficient mapping. Use the words in copied/dictated sentences and paragraphs.
Have students compose sentences and paragraphs containing the words. Review all the words regularly. Break the habit of issuing the words on Monday and testing on Friday, never to be looked at again.

Meaning is key

Define and use each word in a sentence when teaching them. If you don’t provide definitions, the words might as well be in Swahili or Dothraki. How will students develop their awareness of the writing system otherwise? Words are related to one another, and those relationships are driven by meaning. Divorce words from their definitions and you also sever the path to the full richness of the writing system.

How low-quality literacy instruction leads to poor spelling

Due to the fact that letters in words appear in sequences, any instructional method that takes attention away from the sequences will interfere with efficient orthographic mapping. Some widely used examples are the following:
  • Students are sometimes encouraged to guess words by looking away from unfamiliar words, especially if they are prompted to look at pictures instead of the word. Looking at the letters in the words in the correct sequence is not only the habit of good readers, but is also the process th...

Table of contents