Fraser's The Behaviour and Welfare of the Horse
eBook - ePub

Fraser's The Behaviour and Welfare of the Horse

Christopher B Riley, Sharon E Cregier, Andrew Fraser, Christopher B Riley, Sharon E Cregier, Andrew Fraser

Share book
  1. 264 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Fraser's The Behaviour and Welfare of the Horse

Christopher B Riley, Sharon E Cregier, Andrew Fraser, Christopher B Riley, Sharon E Cregier, Andrew Fraser

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

This book continues to provide a comprehensive overview of equine behaviour and an outline of current advances in our understanding, as well as offering insights into contemporary and future challenges for improving horse welfare and safety.Completely updated and revised, a new, international, expert editorial team builds on Andrew Fraser's decades of work as an ethologist, veterinarian, historian, horseman, breeder, trainer, conservationist, and field scientist, sharing essential knowledge to improve horse behaviour and welfare. A range of international experts and key opinion leaders have updated this edition to include the effects of noise on the horse's welfare, husbandry and grazing management including the identification of harmful plants and issues of climate change on pasture. New illustrations and examples bring the book to life and further help to explain equine behaviour in a whole range of different situations, including road transport and horse safety during transport. The text covers key issues concerning equipment and the horse's mouth. It gives new insights into genetics, temperament and horse vocalisations and what these indicate. Welfare assessment models are outlined and the challenges presented in different equestrian sports debated. Difficult topics such as euthanasia are also covered. This classic text remains an essential resource for veterinarians, animal scientists, equine professionals and horse owners.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Fraser's The Behaviour and Welfare of the Horse an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Fraser's The Behaviour and Welfare of the Horse by Christopher B Riley, Sharon E Cregier, Andrew Fraser, Christopher B Riley, Sharon E Cregier, Andrew Fraser in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Medicine & Equine Veterinary Science. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2022
ISBN
9781789242133

1 Development and Assessment of Equine Behaviour

NATALIE K. WARAN PHD1*, GEMMA PEARSON BVMS, MSCR, MRCVS2 AND ANDREW F. FRASER MVSC, MRCVS3
1Eastern Institute of Technology, Hawke’s Bay, New Zealand; 2The University of Edinburgh, Midlothian, Scotland, UK; 3Carbonear, Newfoundland, Canada
The Process of Evolution
Innate Behavioural Patterns and Environment
Changes in Form or Type
Initial Learning
Domestication
Inherent Capability in Learning
Homeostasis and Survival
Appraisal of Behaviour in Horses
Methods for Appraising Behaviour
Open-field tests
Specific behavioural tests used in veterinary studies
Critical Appraisal of Behaviour
Behaviours Associated with Clinical Conditions
Locomotory Behaviour
Ontogeny of Behaviour: Instinct
Instinctive behaviour
Release of innate responses
Sensitive periods and imprinting
Early Experience
Ontogeny of Behaviour: Learning
Classical conditioning
Operant conditioning
Non-associative learning: habituation and desensitisation
Motivation, Reinforcement and Punishment
Emotionality and Intelligence
Cognitive Bias
Behaviour and Equine Welfare
One welfare
References

The Process of Evolution

When the behaviour of the domestic horse is studied, both objectively and realistically, ethology is being applied to a subject that is not entirely a product of natural selection. Variations have been used in the creation of breeds by generations of horse breeders. Nevertheless, this animal’s great inheritance is its natural past, which deserves appreciation because that is the source of much of its behaviour today (Clutton-Brock, 1999).
Darwin concluded that the evolution of a species occurred through selective processes (Jones, 2001). These determine which individuals are most able to survive and reproduce, and consequently to participate in the continuation of the species. The norms of horse behaviour today are the natural heritage of a long history of evolution before human control. Horses were subjected to nature’s control for some 8 million years before their domestication. During those times there was natural selection, in which some individuals were ultimately more successful at living and breeding than others, depending on their genes. This is the process of differential reproduction, whereby evolution occurs as a result. Ethology has an interest in natural selection. Behavioural and physical (i.e. visible) characteristics of a living thing (phenotype) must be distinguished from its genetic make-up (genotype). The evolutionary process produces changes if phenotype and genotype are both involved and adaptations, or differences in traits, increase the fitness of its carrier.
What is selected is the whole make-up of the living organism, including its variations and genes, and its behavioural fit into its ecological niche. The state of overall fitness is achieved through behaviour, structure and niche, and their constant interaction with each other. Applied ethology is interested in the animal’s dynamic wholeness. The applied ethological perspective of evolution is both holistic and traditional. It recognises that behaviour and structure have their own environmental needs and motivational components for survival (Lorenz, 1965).

Innate Behavioural Patterns and Environment

Horses are born equipped with ‘ingrained’ or, more accurately, instinctive responses that provide them with survival advantages. These are fixed action patterns of behaviour carried through generations in the species’ genetic code. These inherited behavioural attributes are common to all individuals in a species, although there may be differences in how these are expressed within individuals. Fixed behaviour patterns are not variations but are ‘fixed’ in the given species – possibly as endpoints of evolution, serving as cornerstones in the long-term support of the species. They are not learned and consist of certain complex responses usually triggered by a simple sensory signal. An example of this is the foal’s responses immediately after being born, such as standing, teat-seeking and sucking from the teat, and then when on its feet, following the dam. All are triggered by simple sign stimuli such as the feel of the mare licking it, the attraction the foal has to the smell of the inguinal wax (hence the term ‘waxing up’) between the teats and a heightened sensitivity to the temperature gradient that will attract them to the right place for milk. Once near the teat, the foal is instinctively attracted to objects of the ‘right’ shape. Once on the teat, the ‘suck reflex’ ensures that the foal gets colostrum quickly to aid his survival. The foal is also born with an instinctive attraction for large moving objects (usually the mare) and will follow her once able to move independently. This means that under natural circumstances, the mare and foal can very quickly re-join the herd.
Table 1.1. Classification of equine species.
Classification
Species
Common names
Order Perissodactyla (odd-toed ungulates)
Equus caballus
Horse (domestic)
Suborder Hippomorpha
Equus asinus
African wild ass (and domestic donkey)
Superfamily Equoidea Family Equidae
Equus burchelli
Common zebra

Changes in Form or Type

Novel features in a species can arise from genetic changes, such as mutations, occurring during cell division in the reproductive process. Such a variety might or might not be suited to the parental niche. If forced to move to a new niche, where it becomes geographically isolated, the mutant character would be perpetuated in homozygous form. This would allow a new form of the species to continue to reproduce unchanged. This phenomenon occurs in many species, including Equidae (see Table 1.1). The present array of horse types suggests that this has occurred periodically, resulting in phenotypic differences, including for example, ponies, heavy horses and the so-called ‘hot-blooded’ horse breeds (see Fig. 1.1 for varietal extremes). Through mutation, the latter type of horse, notably the Arabian, became more reactive and more athletic compared with the ‘cold-blooded’ type. Archetypal characters (both physical and behavioural) are retained and persist in a mutant. This genetic inheritance contributes to the success of the mutant form as a derivative with archetypal attributes. The origin of archetypal behavioural attributes lies in the phenomenon of epigenesis, whereby fixed designs are accurately and actively able to continue their developmental functions through successive generations. For example, a newborn foal, transferred suddenly from an intrauterine environment to a completely new environment to which it must quickly adapt, is equipped with ‘fixed’ action patterns, such as standing and finding the teat, both being essential instinctive behaviours for foal survival (Robinson, 2007).
A photo of a person holding the legs of a young American miniature horse.
Fig. 1.1. Varietal extremes. Young American miniature horse. Photo: C.B. Riley.

Initial Learning

During the early development phase, the mare and her newborn react to one another instinctively and reciprocally. The birthing process triggers the onset of maternal behaviour, and the smell and taste of the uterine membranes and fluids attract the parturient mare to lick the foal clear of membranes and clean of fluids. While doing so, the sensorium of the foal is being ‘imprinted’ with the mare’s details. Thereafter, it efficiently recognizes and remembers significant details about her. Similarly, the mare quickly identifies her progeny and accepts it as something to be protected. The evolved and innate abilities to learn quickly are thereby well illustrated in both neonate and adult.
Evolutionary processes dictate that parents care only for their own offspring, and reproduction occurs only between members of the same species. Such attachments in horses depend on the critical timing of first impressions in the neonatal foal (Bateson and Horn, 1994). The timing of the sensitive period during which the mother–offspring bond is formed is closely matched to the period when the possibility of error is very small, such as before the newborn has a chance to meet members of any species other than that of the mother.
The neonate foal relates to its mother not as a complex organism but as a unitary stimulus. Parturient mares usually isolate themselves from the herd before giving birth. This means that the first large moving object seen by the newborn will be its own dam. Although the foal can be quite easily confused by other large moving objects, it also receives, through touch, ‘thigmotaxic’ stimuli that enable it to recognize its dam. Through the contact of its muzzle with the ventral surfaces of the dam’s body, it learns the identity and location of this vitally important region and eventually finds the dam’s udder and food (see Fig. 1.2). The mare’s behaviour becomes gradually modified through experience to increase the efficiency of her task of watching over the foal, thus granting her reproductive success (see Fig. 1.3).
A photo of a mare licking the body of the newborn foal lying on grassland.
Fig. 1.2. Tactile exploration of the newborn foal by the mare. Reciprocity by the foal will follow once she is standing. Photo: C.B. Riley.
A painting of a mare cuddling its foal by touching the foal's neck with its neck.
Fig. 1.3. Mare with reproductive success. Anonymous artist.

Domestication

Variation through selection as a result of domestication leads to the realization that many genetic mutations are non-adaptive in the evolutionary sense. Breeds with non-adaptive characters have appeared and have been kept true by continuous human selection for specific useful attributes, with the possessors of such mutations being complemented by human agency so that they can survive and reproduce. There are many non-adaptive phenotypic variations, such as coat colour, that play no essential part in the survival of the domesticated species. The true unit is the species as a whole, including variations, as well as the environmental niche and incidental factors within that niche.
For the complete success of the species, behavioural development in relation to environmental pressures play a central role in ensuring that the animal is able to adapt during its own lifetime. Environmental features provided by the husbandry system exert influences that determine whether the horse can adapt behaviourally to the system. Various environmental circumstances have been studied. Changes in behaviour, and ultimately in physical conditions, result directly or indirectly from environmental adaptation. The adaptive mechanisms of horses living under various environmental conditions have shown differences in response to housing, enclosure and the immediate environment. Some responses are maladaptive, and some vary between breeds and individuals. Adaptation is evident in respect of thermoregulation and ingestion (Herdt, 2007).
The temperature at which the so-called cold-blooded type of horse maintains a normal body surface temperature (the thermo-neutral zone) seems to be roughly from −10 to +20 °C, whereas warm-blooded horses can tolerate higher temperatures up to +30 °C. Ranges will be affected by other external factors such as wind and rain. Under extremes of temperature, horses use behavioural methods in attempting to control their temperatures. The most easily recognizable evidence of adaptive behaviour in horses is the movements directed towards thermoregulation and related aspects of body care. These include sheltering, shade-seeking, inactivity, etc. An early behavioural adaptive change during high temperatures is a reduction in food intake. This may arise not only from the shade-seeking activities of the animal but also from a reduced desire for food. Consequent results are an alteration in grazing behaviour and a general decline in all activity. Such an alteration in behaviour may indicate a welfare problem (Dawkins, 2004).
Taste and imitation play important parts in grazing behaviour, and this ensures that the requisite elements are consumed by a horse in appropriate quantities and suitable type to maintain health (McGreevy, 2004). Adaptive seasonal changes in eating indicate that grass palatability has been altered. Seasonal palatability varies markedly on ‘unimproved’ pasture. Horses generally prefer grass that is high in leafiness, greenness and nitrogen content. Sugar, organic acids and fats are preferred, whereas tannins and alkaloids are avoided. Variation in food choice by ponies is explained by the abundance of plant species at different stages of the growing season. As a rule, a drop in the percentage of time spent feeding on a species of grass can be attributed to a dearth of that grass or to the growth of a preferred one. The exceptions to this rule occur in summer and autumn, when certain leaves, such as beech (in the Northern Hemisphere), are increasingly consumed by horses. In the Camargue, in southern France, a long-term study of horses has shown a marked seasonality in ...

Table of contents