The Scientist's Guide to Writing
eBook - ePub

The Scientist's Guide to Writing

How to Write More Easily and Effectively throughout Your Scientific Career

Stephen B. Heard

Share book
  1. 320 pages
  2. English
  3. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  4. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

The Scientist's Guide to Writing

How to Write More Easily and Effectively throughout Your Scientific Career

Stephen B. Heard

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

A concise and accessible primer on the scientific writer's craft The ability to write clearly is critical to any scientific career. The Scientist's Guide to Writing provides practical advice to help scientists become more effective writers so that their ideas have the greatest possible impact.Drawing on his own experience as a scientist, graduate adviser, and editor, Stephen Heard emphasizes that the goal of all scientific writing should be absolute clarity; that good writing takes deliberate practice; and that what many scientists need are not long lists of prescriptive rules but rather direct engagement with their behaviors and attitudes when they write. He combines advice on such topics as how to generate and maintain writing momentum with practical tips on structuring a scientific paper, revising a first draft, handling citations, responding to peer reviews, managing coauthorships, and more.In an accessible, informal tone, The Scientist's Guide to Writing explains essential techniques that students, postdoctoral researchers, and early-career scientists need to write more clearly, efficiently, and easily.

  • Emphasizes writing as a process, not just a product
  • Encourages habits that improve motivation and productivity
  • Explains the structure of the scientific paper and the function of each part
  • Provides detailed guidance on submission, review, revision, and publication
  • Addresses issues related to coauthorship, English as a second language, and more

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is The Scientist's Guide to Writing an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access The Scientist's Guide to Writing by Stephen B. Heard in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Ciencias biológicas & Ciencias en general. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

PART I
image
What Writing Is
Scientists spend enormous amounts of time writing. Many spend more time on writing than on designing experiments, gathering and analyzing data, devising proofs, or any of the other things scientists do. And yet, many scientists pay little attention to writing as a process. They think of it as a rather mechanical step in which they simply record the results of the work after they’re “done.”
This view of writing is badly misleading. For most of us, writing is hard work, a source of stress and frustration, and so it deserves the same kind of deliberate consideration we give to experimental design. What is it that you’re trying to write, and why do the standard scientific forms you use have the structures, styles, and other attributes they do? What belongs in a manuscript, what doesn’t, and why? What are you actually thinking and doing as you sit at the keyboard writing (or, perhaps, not writing)? What’s the relationship between the writer and the reader, and how can deliberate thought about that relationship make your writing better?
The central message of this book is that all these questions can be answered, and the quality and quantity of your writing vastly increased, with attention to three points. First, most scientific writers aren’t born geniuses, but develop facility with writing by deliberately practicing the craft. Second, the goal of all scientific writing is clarity: effortless transfer of information or argument from writer to reader. Third, it’s enormously helpful for writers to think consciously about their own writing behavior. This book will explore all of these points at length. We’ll begin, though, with something fundamental but often overlooked: if you want to get better at writing, it helps to think about what writing is—by which I mean how we write, why we write that way, and how that “way” has evolved over the years to better suit our needs as writers and as readers.
ONE
image
On Bacon, Hobbes, and Newton, and the Selfishness of Writing Well
The Invention of Clarity
In the European early modern period (c.1500–1750), everything was changing. The period saw the Protestant Reformation, the introduction of representative democracy, the secularization of political power, and the origins of the sovereign nation-state. It saw globalization of trade in goods and ideas, but also the subjugation of much of the world under European colonization.
Science was transforming itself right alongside religion, politics, and global economies. European curiosity cabinets (Figure 1.1) were bulging with specimens returned from overseas exploration and trade: stones, creatures, and artifacts begging to be explained by new ideas in natural science and anthropology. Chemistry took its first steps away from alchemy and toward systematic, rational discovery. Astronomy and physics were revolutionized by painstaking observations and new instruments, and by increasing openness of thought about the data these yielded. Finally, the invention and application of the calculus put mathematics at the center of all the sciences.
But while the content of human knowledge was exploding, another, more important change was taking place. The development of modern scientific methods, professional scientists, scientific societies, learned journals, and (in case you were wondering about the point of this historical excursion) modern-style scientific writing changed the way people acquired and communicated knowledge. In a sense, this was when scientists learned to write—or more particularly, to write with the explicit goal of making their ideas available to a broad scientific community.
image
Figure 1.1 Frontispiece to Ole Worm’s (1655) Museum Wormianum, a catalog of his curiosity cabinet.
Writing to communicate with the scientific community was a big change. Medieval “scientists” (alchemists, for instance) generally thought of themselves as solitary workers who would penetrate nature’s secrets for their own gain. Thus, if they wrote their findings down at all it was to claim priority or to make notes for their own use—and what they wrote was deliberately obscure, even written in code, cryptic symbols, or anagrams, to protect their secrets from their rivals. One of the first proponents of change was Francis Bacon, who criticized this secrecy and argued instead in his 1609 essay De Sapientia Veterum that “perfection of the sciences is to be looked for not from the swiftness or ability of any one inquirer, but from a succession.” In the posthumously published New Atlantis (1627), Bacon described a fictitious research institute–cum–scientific society he called “Salomon’s House”—and he clearly intended his utopian novel to be a proposal for how science should work. In Salomon’s House research progressed because scientists communicated and collaborated with one another. (Bacon might well have been inspired by Islamic science of the 8th and 9th centuries, which flourished, collaboratively, under the Abbasid caliphs Harun al-Rashid and Abu al-Mamun [Lyons 2009].)
Bacon’s concept of Salomon’s House inspired the creation of the Royal Society of London in 1660. Its founders extended his ideas about communication among collaborating scientists to communication with a broad scientific community and even with the curious public. One of those founders was Robert Boyle, who essentially invented a new form of writing: the scientific report, which described the methods and results of an experiment (Pérez-Ramos 1996). Another was Thomas Hobbes, who wrote in the preface to his 1655 work De Corpore, “I distinguish the most common notions by accurate definition, for the avoiding of confusion and obscurity” (xiii)—a goal that seems routine today, but would have been outrageously unconventional in Hobbes’s time. The founding of the Society brought with it the first modern scientific journal, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, which printed scientific reports of the kind pioneered by Boyle, written in the clear language advocated by Hobbes. Just a dozen years later, Thomas Sprat described the organization’s rhetorical philosophy as
a constant resolution, to reject all the amplifications, digressions, and swellings of style … a close, naked, natural way of speaking; positive expressions, clear sense, a native easiness: bringing all things as near the mathematical plainness1, as they can: and preferring the language of artisans, countrymen, and merchants, before that of wits, or scholars. (Sprat 1667, 113).
All this may seem obvious from our modern vantage point, but the transition from medieval secrecy through Bacon and Hobbes to the “clear sense [and] native easiness” of Sprat’s Royal Society was revolutionary. Without this tectonic shift in how science was reported, modern science couldn’t be done. The inventions of the calculus, the telescope, the microscope, and the inductive method (all between 1590 and 1630) were certainly important, but they’re all outweighed in importance by the idea of describing one’s scientific thinking clearly, for all to read.
Of course, no revolution lacks holdouts, and the revolution in scientific communication had a curious one: the famously cranky Isaac Newton, for whom publication remained largely about ensuring credit for his work. For example, he drafted his On Analysis by Infinite Series in 1669 in response to Nicholas Mercator’s Logorithmotechnia, which Newton worried would undermine his claim of first discovery for some key insights underlying the calculus. Despite pressure from colleagues, Newton allowed only limited circulation of the manuscript within the Royal Society; not until 1711 would he agree to open publication. More famously, he deliberately made his masterwork Principia Mathematica—and especially its third volume, De mundi systemate—difficult to read. Newton had originally written De mundi systemate in plain language to be accessible to readers (Westfall 1980, 459) but changed his mind and rewrote it as series of propositions, derivations, lemmas, and proofs comprehensible only to accomplished mathematicians. He left little doubt of his intent, telling his friend William Derham that “in order to avoid being baited by little smatterers in mathematics, he [Newton] designedly made his Principia abstruse” (Derham 1733). That is, he wrote to impede communication with other scientists, not to facilitate it! Of course, by then Newton was a superstar whose writing was likely to command from his readers whatever effort was needed to penetrate the fog. And readers could spare the effort, as the flow of published works competing for scientists’ attention was still little more than a trickle. This, too, would change.
Clarity and “Telepathy” in the Modern Era
Bacon, Hobbes, Sprat, and others of their time were taking the first steps toward what became, by the twentieth century, a consensus that the goal of most writing is clear communication. The best-known reflection of this is probably The Elements of Style, by William Strunk Jr. and E. B. White, first published in 1920. White described Strunk’s opinion that the typical reader was “floundering in a swamp” and that it was “the duty of anyone trying to write English to drain this swamp quickly and get [the reader] up on dry ground, or at least throw [down] a rope” (Strunk and White 1972, xii). However forceful Strunk’s pleading, though, the argument for clarity has its purest expression in Stephen King’s 2000 On Writing: A Memoir of the Craft. King’s chapter “What Writing Is” opens with the simple declaration: “Telepathy, of course” (King 2000, 95).
The word “telepathy” may seem chosen for humor, but in scientific writing your goal should always be communication so crystal clear that it feels to the reader like telepathy—like direct transmission to the reader’s brain from yours. You are writing because you have some information to transmit, and your goal should be for the reader to receive that information without even being aware of the process. As Nathaniel Hawthorne put it, “The greatest possible merit of style is … to make the words absolutely disappear into the thought” (letter to E. A. Duyckinck, 27 Apr. 1851, quoted in Van Doren [1949], 267). If the reader pauses to question your word choice or needs to squint to distinguish between two lines on a graph, then you have joined a battle you don’t want to be in: what you’re trying to say is fighting for the reader’s attention with the way you’re saying it.
At this point you might be a little skeptical. After all, popular wisdom holds that people who use big words and complicated sentences seem more intelligent. (A quick internet search will turn up dozens of lists of “Ten Words That Will Make You Sound Smarter.”) There is even some limited support for this idea: one study compared undergraduates reading texts printed in easier- and harder-to-read fonts (to vary reading difficulty independent of content) and found they scored the harder-to-read texts as better written (Galak and Nelson 2011). However, most research finds the opposite: that people ascribe higher intelligence to writers who (and higher quality to texts that) use clearer fonts, smaller words, and simpler sentences (e.g., Oppenheimer 2006). Even if difficult prose did make you seem smarter, this would only help if people actually read it—which brings me to my next point.
The Selfishness of Writing Well
Achieving telepathic writing is hard work (chapter 2). I have spent many hundreds of hours crafting pieces of writing that I hoped might achieve crystal clarity, and in this book I will urge you to do the same. Those were hundreds of hours I could have spent doing more experiments, or drinking beer with friends, or even just walking along the water’s edge skipping stones. So why invest the time and effort in writing well?
It might seem that working to make your writing clear is an act of selfless generosity toward the reader—this is the impression left by Strunk’s metaphor of throwing the reader a rescue line. Or it might seem an act of generosity toward the progress of science. This was the argument made by Bacon, Sprat, and others in the 1600s; in this view, Newton was selfish in withholding his written work and writing for opacity. There’s no question that writing well serves both the reader and the progress of science. But the evolution of science since Newton’s time, and especially its spectacular growth, has changed the incentives for writing well.
In the 1680s, Newton had the luxury of writing a difficult book and knowing that every mathematician, physicist, and astronomer who mattered would invest whatever time was needed to grapple with his text. There just weren’t many works of similar importance competing for their attention. But in our modern era, the deluge of published scientific work becomes greater every year. Just for the year 2012, for example, a Web of Science search returns more than sixty thousand records for oncology alone, fifty-one thousand for physical chemistry, and forty-five thousand for optics. By comparison, the mere nine thousand records for virology seem almost manageable—but even if you considered just ten percent of the virology literature relevant to your own work, keeping up with it would mean reading three papers every single day of the year. That might be possible for a while, but these numbers include only peer-reviewed papers in journals indexed by the WoS, not papers in more obscure journals, technical reports, books, book chapters, theses, grant proposals, or any of the other forms of scientific writing that form teetering piles in scientists’ offices around the globe.
As a scientific writer, then, you are competing for attention with an incredible array of material your reader might prefer to your own. Your career and reputation, though, depend on having your work read. Hiring, promotion, and tenure committees and granting councils devour citation data for your publications. Grad-school admissions committees look for evidence of writing skill, and the best prospective graduate students search for supervisors by reading the literature to find someone whose ideas excite them. And, of course, journal editors and reviewers groan under the weight of submitted manuscripts, and can’t be depended on to see the jewel hidden in a manuscript that’s difficult to read. Readers have a lot to choose from. If your paper isn’t clear they will turn to another. When they do, it’s you as the writer who suffers most.
You can’t make your reader like your science simply by writing better—but you can make it easier for them to see why they should like it, or at least why they should read and cite it. Given...

Table of contents