Developing Reading Comprehension
eBook - ePub

Developing Reading Comprehension

Paula J. Clarke, Emma Truelove, Charles Hulme, Margaret J. Snowling

Share book
  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Developing Reading Comprehension

Paula J. Clarke, Emma Truelove, Charles Hulme, Margaret J. Snowling

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

Developing Reading Comprehension

"In recent years the debate about teaching young children to read has tended to focus upon equipping them with the crucially important knowledge and skills they need to read words accurately in and out of context, that is to say, teaching them how the alphabet works for reading and spelling. While such knowledge and skills are essential, more is required for children to become literate, fluent readers who understand what they read. In short, the goal of reading is comprehension. This book scrupulously examines the obstacles to reading comprehension and exemplifies what can be done to help children overcome them. It is an important and timely contribution to securing high-quality teaching of the range of attributes children need to become fully-fledged readers."

Sir Jim Rose, CBE

"The studies by Professors Charles Hulme and Maggie Snowling and their team over two decades based around the Reading Intervention Programme are the most sustained, comprehensive and rigorous research series on reading yet conducted in the UK. Their increasing focus on children who experience the most difficulty in reading is exactly where attention should be directed. This volume summarises the team's achievements to date, and is most eagerly awaited."

Greg Brooks, Emeritus Professor of Education, University of Sheffield, Member of European High Level Group of Experts on Literacy

"Developing Reading Comprehension presents a landmark study from the top research team in the UK on how to improve reading comprehension. It's an exemplary masters-level textbook written with undergraduate-level lucidity and approachability."

Colin Harrison, Emeritus Professor of Literacy Studies in Education, University of Nottingham

A significant minority of children aged 7–11, despite being able to read fluently and accurately, have difficulty extracting meaning from text. This detailed guide offers three evidence-based intervention programmes, drawn from the cutting edge of educational psychology, for improving the reading skills of children in this group. It includes a definitive introduction to the characteristics of the 'poor comprehender profile', and explains how to monitor and assess students' experiences and learning outcomes. With invaluable strategies for teachers, psychologists and special educational needs coordinators, the book will help professionals to support learners in their efforts to explore the full richness of language and to read with real understanding.

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Developing Reading Comprehension an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Developing Reading Comprehension by Paula J. Clarke, Emma Truelove, Charles Hulme, Margaret J. Snowling in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Education & Teaching Methods for Reading. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Year
2013
ISBN
9781118606742

Chapter 1

What is Reading Comprehension?

Language conveys meaning and allows the sharing of information, ideas and perspectives. When written messages are successfully understood, reading can be a wonderfully inspiring, enjoyable and transforming experience. Written language has the power to take the mind to different places, times and events; it can put us in the shoes of fascinating characters and hold our attention through gripping plots, suspense and intrigue. Texts can provide escapism and offer alternative perspectives on the world; what’s more, they can ‘kindle’ our imaginations to create rich mental images that may stay with us forever. Texts can inform and develop knowledge, provide us with new vocabulary and provoke new ways of thinking.
For many children, however, the messages conveyed through written text are not well understood; this has potentially far-reaching consequences for their learning, development and well-being. This chapter outlines the richness of written language and the complexities of the processes involved in reading for meaning. This serves to highlight the many ways in which children’s ability to understand text can break down, and will provide points to consider when teaching and developing interventions to improve reading comprehension. Chapter 2 considers in more detail the difficulties that cause some children to have specific ­difficulties in understanding what they read.
To consider the richness of written language, let us consider the ­following short passage as an example:
Jennie sprang bolt upright. Moments of disorientation followed before she recognised the now faded floral wallpaper and tatty matching ­curtains. Framed family faces stared down from the bookshelf. ‘Home for the holidays’ she remembered. Blinking and yawning she stumbled around for her slippers and gown. The tinny voice from the ­bedside table was delivering the news and warning of harsh winds and icy roads. Jennie reached across and hit the button. ‘Today is definitely a two sweater day,’ Jennie thought as she rifled around in her suitcase for her favourite winter clothing.
Reflect for a moment on your understanding of the passage and think about the following questions:
Why did Jennie spring bolt upright? Where was Jennie? Why might she have felt disorientated? What are the framed family faces? Why did she stumble? Where was the tinny voice coming from? What was the button? What did Jennie mean by a ‘two sweater day’? What was Jennie doing in her suitcase?
Returning to the passage, consider your experience of trying to ­decipher its meaning. In the annotated version in Figure 1.1 we have documented some of the initial reflections that you may have when you reread the passage.
The complexity of comprehension is highlighted in these annotations. Engaging in this type of conscious ‘think-aloud’ activity focuses attention on aspects of reading comprehension that might otherwise go unnoticed. In the example, you can see that our understanding of the text’s message gradually builds up over the course of reading it. Initial predictions are confirmed by later information, potentially ambiguous vocabulary is resolved by the context and assumptions based on previous experience are tested. The annotations, however, only scratch the surface of the demands of the task. Making connections between parts of the passage in order to build up an interpretation requires recognition of the words, an ability to hold information in mind, an ability to scan backwards and forwards to relevant words and phrases, an understanding of cues from sentence structure and punctuation, an empathy with the character and many other skills and processes.
Only part of the task of reading comprehension is situated within the text itself; a developed understanding comes from the interaction between the text and the reader’s response to it. The diverse perspectives that we bring to the task result in different interpretations of a text. When we watch films of books that we have read they rarely match up to our imagined versions. When we discuss reading material with others, we may find that we have interpreted the same sentence in very different ways. We may also find that our interpretations are inconsistent with the message that was intended by the author. Such differences in imagination and personal response, whilst complex and difficult to capture, are at the heart of the reading comprehension experience.
Figure 1.1 An annotated version of the sample passage demonstrating the results of a think-aloud activity
image

MODELS OF READING COMPREHENSION

Models of reading comprehension can help us to understand the ­different skills and processes involved in interpreting text. The simple view of reading (Gough and Tunmer, 1986) offers a useful model for characterising successful reading. As shown in Figure 1.2, Gough and Tunmer (1986) propose that two skills are needed in order to read for meaning: the ability to recognise or pronounce the words (decoding) and the ability to understand spoken language (listening comprehension).
Figure 1.2 The simple view of reading (Gough and Tunmer, 1986)
image
As well as helping us to understand reading success, this model can help to describe the different ways in which reading can break down and inform early identification and intervention for children at risk of reading difficulties. According to the simple model, a child may show a generally poor reading profile and experience difficulties in developing both decoding and listening comprehension. Alternatively, a child may have difficulties in the area of decoding but show intact or even superior listening comprehension skills. The opposite pattern to this, termed ‘the poor comprehender profile’, is also possible and will be the focus of the next chapter. This profile is characterised by intact or superior decoding skills coupled with weak listening comprehension. As a result, children with a poor comprehender reading profile will read aloud well but have difficulty understanding what they read. Children who experience difficulties with decoding, listening comprehension or both skills will experience difficulties in understanding text.
Another model that can be used to capture the skills and processes involved in successful reading is the Construction–Integration Model of Kintsch and Rawson (2005). This model, presented in Figure 1.3, provides a more detailed overview of the processes involved in reading comprehension. The model proposes that when we read text we create a personal representation of its meaning; this representation (or mental model) is made up of the information from the text itself alongside our general knowledge of the words and the topic.
The processes involved in deciphering the text are described in terms of three levels. The first is the ‘linguistic’ level in which the reader ­recognises and processes individual words and their meanings. The second is the ‘microstructure’ in which the reader goes beyond words in isolation to recognise and process the meaning of larger chunks of text. The third is the ‘macrostructure’ in which the reader recognises and processes themes, topics and genre information about the text. These three levels form what is called a ‘textbase’. The textbase ­combines with the reader’s existing general knowledge to form an individual’s representation of the meaning of the text. This interpretation is called the ‘situation model’ and, in forming it, the reader uses many different types of knowledge. For example, the reader may need to use theory of mind skills to understand the author’s or character’s intentions, beliefs, desires and feelings. To have a ‘theory of mind’ requires an awareness of your own mind, recognition that other people have minds and an understanding that another’s mind is independent of your own. This understanding provides the foundation for perspective taking and the realisation that others have ideas and emotions which may be similar to or different from your own.
Figure 1.3 The Construction–Integration Model (Kintsch and Rawson, 2005)
image
image
To illustrate the application of this model, we can link its features to the processes involved in understanding the passage introduced at the beginning of this chapter. Let’s remind ourselves of the passage:
Jennie sprang bolt upright. Moments of disorientation followed before she recognised the now faded floral wallpaper and tatty matching curtains. Framed family faces stared down from the bookshelf. ‘Home for the holidays’ she remembered. Blinking and yaw[ning] she stumbled around for her slippers and gown. The tinny voice from the bedside table was delivering the news and warning of harsh winds and icy roads. Jennie reached across and hit the button. ‘Today is definitely a two sweater day,’ Jennie thought as she rifled around in her suitcase for her favourite winter clothing.
By following this example through, you can see how an apparently simple passage can be understood at several different levels, all of which contribute to the formation of a rich situation model.
Taken together, these models describe many of the skills and processes involved in reading comprehension. However, it is important to remember that models are not ‘real’ – they are theories that may be incomplete or incorrect in a variety of ways. Such models can be tested and refined in research into how people read. Consideration should also be paid to other aspects of reading not specified in these models, for example the relationship between understanding text and motivation. When we read something that we understand and can immerse ourselves in, we are motivated to read more. Enjoyment of text ­promotes engagement and sustained interest and attention. If comprehension breaks down reading will be less pleasurable, which may lead to less time spent reading, which in turn will weaken reading skills further.
It is also necessary to recognise the metacognitive skills that underlie reading for meaning. Metacognition refers to what we know about our own knowledge and includes the ability to reflect on our understanding of text as it unfolds. As we saw in the ‘think-aloud’ activity at the beginning of this chapter, there are many aspects of reading comprehension that can be dissected and reflected upon. As individuals, we vary in our ability to keep track of what and how much we understand. Some people may think frequently about their interpretation of a text both during and after reading, whilst others may monitor less and may continue to read on, long after they have lost the thread of the story or passage. The extent to which an individual monitors their own understanding may also vary according to the reason why they are reading. For example, if you have been asked to prepare for an exam, you may read more intently and with more emphasis on understanding than if you are flicking through a magazine in the dentist’s waiting room. This aspect of reading for meaning is often referred to by the term ‘standard of coherence’; that is the degree to which an individual is concerned with whether text makes sense.
Perfetti, Landi and Oakhill (2005) argue that for reading comprehension to develop, a high standard of coherence is necessary. To give an example, Sophia and George are two children with different standards of coherence. When reading a story, they may extract the same level of meaning but while Sophia classes this as unclear, George considers it is satisfactory and therefore does not seek further clarification. Sophia is likely to be better able to develop good comprehension because she deploys a number of strategies to gain further information that will enable her to overcome the feeling of not fully understanding; these may include asking an adult or a peer a follow-up question or thinking back to a similar personal experience and bringing her previous knowledge to bear. George, on the other hand, may read on without the sense that he has not fully extracted the meaning from the passage.

THE IMPORTANCE OF READING COMPREHENSION

At this point, it is necessary to return to the importance of reading for meaning. Much of the commentary surrounding theoretical models of reading comprehension has focused on the knowledge that the reader brings to the process of understanding. However, it is important to recognise that reading can be a transformative experience influencing the thinking and learning of the reader. New words, concepts and perspectives can be encountered that challenge and enhance existing knowledge. Consequently, reading is central to teaching and learning and it is vital to consider the circumstances in which the developing child is required to extract and apply meaning derived from text.
Reading comprehension skills become more important as children progress through the educational system. Teachers frequently expect children and young people to research topics from books or from the internet in the Sciences as well as the Arts and Humanities. Indeed, in all areas of the curriculum children need to be able to locate relevant information, to filter out the information that is less pertinent to the ­current topic and to select the appropriate information to focus upon. For example, maths comprehension exercises draw upon reading ­comprehension skills to support the development of numeracy skills. Consider the following:
The local ...

Table of contents