The Neuroethology of Predation and Escape
eBook - ePub

The Neuroethology of Predation and Escape

Keith T. Sillar, Laurence D. Picton, William J. Heitler

Partager le livre
  1. English
  2. ePUB (adapté aux mobiles)
  3. Disponible sur iOS et Android
eBook - ePub

The Neuroethology of Predation and Escape

Keith T. Sillar, Laurence D. Picton, William J. Heitler

DĂ©tails du livre
Aperçu du livre
Table des matiĂšres
Citations

À propos de ce livre

THE NEUROETHOLOGY OF PREDATION AND ESCAPE

To eat and not get eaten is key to animal survival, and the arms race between predators and prey has driven the evolution of many rapid and spectacular behaviours.

This book explores the neural mechanisms controlling predation and escape, where specialisations in afferent pathways, central circuits, motor control and biomechanics can be traced through to natural animal behaviour.

Each chapter provides an integrated and comparative review of case studies in neuroethology. Ranging from the classic studies on bat biosonar and insect counter-measures, through to fish-eating snails armed with powerful neurotoxins, the book covers a diverse and fascinating range of adaptations. Common principles of biological design and organization are highlighted throughout the text.

The book is aimed at several audiences:

  • for lecturers and students. This synthesis will help to underpin the curriculum in neuroscience and behavioural biology, especially for courses focusing on neuroethology
  • for postgraduate students. The sections devoted to your area of specialism will give a flying start to your research reading, while the other chapters offer breadth and insights from comparative studies
  • for academic researchers. The book will provide a valuable resource and an enjoyable read

Above all, we hope this book will inspire the next generation of neuroethologists.

Foire aux questions

Comment puis-je résilier mon abonnement ?
Il vous suffit de vous rendre dans la section compte dans paramĂštres et de cliquer sur « RĂ©silier l’abonnement ». C’est aussi simple que cela ! Une fois que vous aurez rĂ©siliĂ© votre abonnement, il restera actif pour le reste de la pĂ©riode pour laquelle vous avez payĂ©. DĂ©couvrez-en plus ici.
Puis-je / comment puis-je télécharger des livres ?
Pour le moment, tous nos livres en format ePub adaptĂ©s aux mobiles peuvent ĂȘtre tĂ©lĂ©chargĂ©s via l’application. La plupart de nos PDF sont Ă©galement disponibles en tĂ©lĂ©chargement et les autres seront tĂ©lĂ©chargeables trĂšs prochainement. DĂ©couvrez-en plus ici.
Quelle est la différence entre les formules tarifaires ?
Les deux abonnements vous donnent un accĂšs complet Ă  la bibliothĂšque et Ă  toutes les fonctionnalitĂ©s de Perlego. Les seules diffĂ©rences sont les tarifs ainsi que la pĂ©riode d’abonnement : avec l’abonnement annuel, vous Ă©conomiserez environ 30 % par rapport Ă  12 mois d’abonnement mensuel.
Qu’est-ce que Perlego ?
Nous sommes un service d’abonnement Ă  des ouvrages universitaires en ligne, oĂč vous pouvez accĂ©der Ă  toute une bibliothĂšque pour un prix infĂ©rieur Ă  celui d’un seul livre par mois. Avec plus d’un million de livres sur plus de 1 000 sujets, nous avons ce qu’il vous faut ! DĂ©couvrez-en plus ici.
Prenez-vous en charge la synthÚse vocale ?
Recherchez le symbole Écouter sur votre prochain livre pour voir si vous pouvez l’écouter. L’outil Écouter lit le texte Ă  haute voix pour vous, en surlignant le passage qui est en cours de lecture. Vous pouvez le mettre sur pause, l’accĂ©lĂ©rer ou le ralentir. DĂ©couvrez-en plus ici.
Est-ce que The Neuroethology of Predation and Escape est un PDF/ePUB en ligne ?
Oui, vous pouvez accĂ©der Ă  The Neuroethology of Predation and Escape par Keith T. Sillar, Laurence D. Picton, William J. Heitler en format PDF et/ou ePUB ainsi qu’à d’autres livres populaires dans Biological Sciences et Zoology. Nous disposons de plus d’un million d’ouvrages Ă  dĂ©couvrir dans notre catalogue.

Informations

Éditeur
Wiley-Blackwell
Année
2016
ISBN
9781118527238
Édition
1
Sous-sujet
Zoology

CHAPTER 1
Vision

The Neuroethology of Predation and Escape
An illustration of a toad with dashed lines from its eyes connecting to the worm on the ground and a schematic diagram of converging pathways involved in visual predation in toads.

Very few species of animal are active only in complete darkness. At any moment in time, half of our planet is bathed in light radiating from the sun, while the other half remains partially lit by reflections from the moon, distant starlight, bioluminescence and human light pollution. It is thus unsurprising that many animals make use of some form of vision to provide information about the environment. Vision often provides the earliest cues for the detection of a predator or prey; it usually has a longer range than hearing, and is more precise and immediate than smell. Even when other sensory modalities do take the lead, vision is almost always additionally involved in the pursuit of prey, the guidance of a predatory strike or the coordination of an escape manoeuvre.
The information provided by vision depends on the complexity and sophistication of the eye, but even the simplest eyes are adept at detecting changes in light intensity, and these temporal cues are often important signals warning of the approach of danger. Many animals, particularly sessile forms such as barnacles, tubeworms or bivalve molluscs, display a shadow withdrawal reflex in response to a sudden dimming of the light. In these cases, the image detail is irrelevant, and it is the overall level of environmental illumination that matters. However, for many other animals, the image is crucial, because it enables the eye’s owner to determine where the object of interest is located. Indeed, some authorities argue that it was the evolution of image-forming eyes, and a consequent step increase in the speed of the predator-prey arms race, that led to the massive proliferation in animal diversity about 540 million years ago, known as the Cambrian Explosion (Parker, 1998).
In this chapter, we will cover some of the key case studies in neuroethology that have informed our understanding of how animals use vision to detect, identify and respond to predators and prey. We first outline important principles of visual processing, including properties of light and the concepts of foveae, acuity and sensitivity. We then look at how meaningful features of an image are extracted as information passes through the visual pathway, in order to identify objects as predator or prey, using the toad as a model system. In the next part of the chapter, we outline the neurobiology of infrared vision in snakes. These ambush predators possess a highly specialised infrared visual system used to detect and target their warm-blooded prey. Finally, we will look at specialisations of the visual system in aerial predators that enable them to capture prey while simultaneously coordinating flight, using dragonfly vision as an exquisite example.

1.1 The electromagnetic spectrum

Light is a form of electromagnetic radiation (Figure 1.1) that consists of waves of energy that radiate through the air1 at roughly 300 000 km/s. The intensity of light is determined by the amplitude of these waves, while what we perceive as colour is encoded in their wavelength.
Electromagnetic (EM) spectrum depicting the range of wavelengths from radio waves (low energy) with long wavelength to gamma rays (high‐energy) with short wavelengths.
Figure 1.1 The electromagnetic (EM) spectrum. EM radiation spans a range of wavelengths, from low-energy, long wavelength radio waves to high-energy, short wavelength gamma rays. Most animals detect light only within a very narrow range of wavelengths; for example, the ‘visible spectrum’ in humans is between is about 390–700 nm. Some animals are also capable of detecting ultraviolet radiation (insects, birds and fish) and infrared radiation (crotalid and boid snakes, fire-seeking beetles and vampire bats).
For many animals, including humans, visible light spans a range of wavelengths between approximately 390 nm and 700 nm (the visible spectrum; Figure 1.1). We perceive these wavelengths as the colours of the rainbow, and we are able to do so because we possess a trichromatic visual pigment system. The cone cells of the human retina contain photo-pigments that absorb either red, green or blue wavelengths of light that has bounced off objects2 in the environment, and our brains then interpret the mixtures of cone signals as different colours. However, cones have a relatively high response threshold, and thus only respond if the light is quite bright. We possess an additional set of photoreceptors, the rod cells, which absorb light across the full visible spectrum and, thus, are unable to distinguish between different colours, but are very sensitive to low light levels. The rod system therefore only mediates greyscale imaging (and the rod pigments are completely bleached in bright daylight), but it can function at low ambient light levels, in which the cone system is effectively blind. The two systems overlap and operate in parallel at intermediate light levels, such as twilight. Not all animals possess colour vision but, for those that do, it can be very useful in detecting warning coloration, such as aposematic beetles that warn predators they are distasteful.
Many animals are able to detect wavelengths of light beyond the spectrum to which humans are restricted (Figure 1.1). For example, many insects, birds and fish can visualise shorter wavelengths of light in the ultraviolet (UV) range, and they do so to detect a range of important ecological features, from enticing landing strips on flowers (bees) to the detection of aerial insect prey against the sky (dragonflies)3. One of the few mammals that can see in UV is the arctic reindeer (caribou), and here the key advantage seems to be contrast enhancement (Hogg et al., 2011). The ground is often snow-covered, and predators such as wolves, with white fur, are hard to detect in the normal visible spectrum. However, snow is a strong reflector of UV, while fur is a strong absorber, which means that, in the UV spectrum, the wolf stands out clearly against a snowy background.
At the other end of the spectrum, some animals can also detect light in the longer wavelength infrared (IR) range (see Campbell et al. (2002) for a general review). Fire-seeking beetles, for instance, can detect IR light emitted by forest fires at a distance of up to 80 km, allowing them to navigate to freshly burnt wood, into which they lay their eggs (Schmitz and Bleckmann, 1998). Vampire bats possess IR-detecting receptors on their facial nose-leaf that are used to locate the most nutritious, blood-rich regions of a target prey animal. However, the most sophisticated IR visual system belongs to crotalid and boid snakes, which includes pit vipers, boas and pythons. These snakes possess highly specialised IR-detecting organs on their snout which allow them to detect, locate and capture their warm-blooded prey, even in what we would consider to be the dark (see Section 1.5).
Many animals, including a number of arthropods and cephalopods, are also highly sensitive to another property of light: polarisation. Mantis shrimp are the most sensitive – they have 12 visual pigments, and not only can they detect UV and IR radiation, but also circularly and linearly polarised light! Polarisation sensitivity is probably mainly used for navigation, but it can also be used for detecting certain types of prey – for instance, squid use polarized light sensitivity to visualise otherwise invisible, transparent zooplankton (Shashar et al., 1998).

1.2 Eyes: acuity and sensitivity

The quality of an eye and, therefore, its usefulness to either predator or prey, strongly depends on two key factors:
  • Sensitivity – the amount of light energy that it can capture from a given source.
  • Resolution (acuity) – the accuracy with which it can determine the spatial origin of that source.
Sensitivity depends partly on receptor physiology, but also on eye str...

Table des matiĂšres