The Sounds of Language
eBook - ePub

The Sounds of Language

An Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology

Elizabeth C. Zsiga

Share book
  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

The Sounds of Language

An Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology

Elizabeth C. Zsiga

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

The Sounds of Language is an introductory guide to the linguistic study of speech sounds, which provides uniquely balanced coverage of both phonology and phonetics.

  • Features exercises and problem sets, as well as supporting online resources at www.wiley.com/go/zsiga, including additional discussion questions and exercises, as well as links to further resources such as sound files, video files, and useful websites
  • Creates opportunities for students to practice data analysis and hypothesis testing
  • Integrates data on sociolinguistic variation, first language acquisition, and second language learning
  • Explores diverse topics ranging from the practical, such as how to make good digital recordings, make a palatogram, solve a phoneme/allophone problem, or read a spectrogram; to the theoretical, including the role of markedness in linguistic theory, the necessity of abstraction, features and formal notation, issues in speech perception as distinct from hearing, and modelling sociolinguistic and other variations
  • Organized specifically to fit the needs of undergraduate students of phonetics and phonology, and is structured in a way which enables instructors to use the text both for a single semester phonetics and phonology course or for a two-course sequence

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is The Sounds of Language an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access The Sounds of Language by Elizabeth C. Zsiga in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Languages & Linguistics & Phonetics & Phonology. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

1
The Vocal Tract
In all things of nature there is something of the marvelous.
Aristotle, Parts of Animals
Parts is parts.
Wendy’s commercial
Chapter outline
1.1 Seeing the vocal tract: tools for speech research
1.2 Parts of the vocal tract
1.2.1 The sub-laryngeal vocal tract
1.2.2 The larynx
1.2.3 The supra-laryngeal vocal tract
Chapter summary
Further reading
Review exercises
Further analysis and discussion
Further research
Go online
References
We begin our study of the sounds of speech by surveying the parts of the body used to make speech sounds: the vocal tract. An understanding of how these parts fit and act together, the topic of Section 1.2, is crucial for everything that comes later in the book. Before we dive into the study of human anatomy, however, Section 1.1 considers some of the tools that speech scientists have used or currently use to do their work: How can we “see” inside the body to know what our vocal tracts are doing?

1.1 Seeing the Vocal Tract: Tools for Speech Research

The vocal tract is comprised of all the parts of the body that are used in the creation of speech sounds, from the abdominal muscles that contract to push air out of the lungs, to the lips and nostrils from which the sound emerges. We sometimes call this collection of parts “the organs of speech,” but there really is no such thing. Every body part that is used for speech has some other biological function – the lungs for breathing, the tongue and teeth for eating, the larynx to close off the lungs and keep the two systems separate – and is only secondarily adapted for speech.
We’re not sure at what point in time the human vocal tract developed its present form, making speech as we know it possible; some scientists estimate it may have been 50,000 to 100,000 years ago. And we don’t know which came first, the development of a complex brain that enables linguistic encoding, or the development of the vocal structures to realize the code in sound. While hominid fossils provide some clues about brain size and head shape, neither brains nor tongues are well preserved in the fossil record. We do know that no other animal has the biological structure needed to make the full range of human speech sounds. Even apes and chimpanzees, whose anatomy is generally similar to ours, have jaws and skulls of very different shape from ours, and could only manage one or two consonants and vowels. That’s why scientists who try to teach language to chimps or apes use manual sign language instead: chimps are much better at manipulating their fingers than their mouths. There are birds that are excellent mimics of human speech sounds, but their “talking” is really a complex whistling, bearing little resemblance to the way that humans create speech. (The exact mechanism used by these birds is discussed in Chapter 9.)
But probably for as long as people have been talking, people have been interested in describing how speech sounds get made. Linguistic descriptions are found among the oldest records of civilization. In Ancient India, as early as 500 BCE, scribes (the most famous of whom was known as P
x101_rn
x1E47_rn
ini) were making careful notes of the exact articulatory configurations required to pronounce the Vedic Scriptures, and creating detailed anatomical descriptions and rules for Sanskrit pronunciation and grammar. (The younger generation, apparently, was getting the pronunciation all wrong.) Arab phoneticians, working several centuries later but with many of the same motivations as the Indian Grammarians, produced extensive descriptions of Classical Arabic. Al-Khalil, working in Basra around 100 CE, produced a 4,000-page dictionary entitled Kitab al ‘ayn, “The Book of the Letter ‘Ayn’.” The ancient Greeks and Romans seemed to be more interested in syntax and logic than in phonetics or phonology, but they also conducted anatomical experiments, engaging in an ongoing debate over the origin of speech in the body. Zeno the Stoic argued that speech must come from the heart, which he understood to be the source of reason and emotion, while Aristotle deduced the sound-producing function of the larynx. The Greek physician Galen seems to have settled the argument in favor of the Aristotelian view by noting that pigs stop squealing when their throats are cut. Medieval European linguists continued in the Greek tradition, further developing Greek ideas on logic and grammar, as well as continuing to study anatomy through dissection.
The main obstacle in studying speech is that the object of study is for the most part invisible. Absent modern tools, studies of speech production had to be based on either introspection or dissection. (According to one history of speech science, it didn’t occur to anyone until 1854 that one could use mirrors to view the living larynx in motion.) Of course some speech movements are visible, especially the lips and jaw and sometimes the front of the tongue, so that “lip reading” is possible, though difficult. But most of speech cannot be seen: the movement of the tongue in the throat, the opening of the passage between nose and mouth, sound waves as they travel through the air, the vibration of the fluid in the inner ear. The experimental techniques of modern speech science almost all involve ways of making these invisible movements visible, and thus measurable.
One obvious way is to take the pieces out, at autopsy. Dissection studies have been done since antiquity, and much important information has been gained this way, such as our knowledge of where muscles and cartilages are located, and how they attach to each other. But the dead patient doesn’t speak. Autopsy can tell us about the anatomy of the vocal tract, that is, the shape and structure of its parts, but it cannot tell us much about physiology, that is, the way the parts work together to produce a specific sound or sound sequence.
The discovery of the X-ray in 1895 was a major advance in speech science, enabling researchers to “see” inside the body. (The mysterious “X” ray was discovered by physicist Wilheim Conrad Röntgen, who received the first Nobel Prize in physics for his work.) X-rays are not necessarily great tools for visualizing the organs of speech, however, for two reasons. X-rays work because they pass through less dense, water-based soft tissue like skin, but are absorbed by denser materials like bone, teeth, and lead. Thus, if an X-ray is passed through the body, the bones cast a white “shadow” on a photographic film placed behind the subject. The first problem with the use of X-rays in speech science is that muscles, like the tongue, are more like skin than like bones. The tongue is visible on an X-ray, but only as a faint cloud, not a definite sharp outline.
Figure 1.1 shows the results of one experiment where researchers tried to get around this problem in an ingenious way. These images were made by the British phonetician Daniel Jones, in 1917. Jones was very interested in vowel sounds, and is famous (among other achievements) for devising a system for describing the sound of any vowel in any language (the “cardinal vowel” system, which is discussed in Chapter 4). But nobody really knew exactly how the tongue moved to make these different sounds, since we cannot see most of the tongue, and we do not have a very good sense of even where our own tongues are as we speak.
Figure 1.1 X-rays from the lab of Daniel Jones.
Source: Published in Jones (1966) The Pronunciation of English.
c01f001
To create these images, Jones swallowed one end of a small lead chain, holding on to the other end, so that the chain lay across his tongue. He then allowed himself to be X-rayed while articulating different vowel sounds, and these pictures are the result. The images, beginning at the upper left and going clockwise, show vowels similar to those in the words “heed,” “who’d,” “hod,” and “had.” (“Hod” rhymes with “rod”, and was a common word in 1917. It refers to a bucket or shovel for carrying coal.) The tongue itself does not image well, but the lead chain shows up beautifully, indicating how the tongue is higher and more towards the front of the mouth for the vowel in “heed” than the vowel in “hod.” (The large dot showing the high point of the tongue was drawn in by hand, and the other black dots are lead fillings in the subject’s teeth.)
The second problem with X-ray technology, of course, is that we eventually learned that absorbing X-rays into your bones, not to mention swallowing lead, is dangerous for the subject. Prof. Jones’ experiments would never make it past the review committees that every university now has in place to protect subjects’ health.
A safe way to get pictures of parts of the vocal tract is through sonography. This technology, based on the reflection of sound waves, was developed in World War II, to allow ships to “see” submarines under the water. Most of us are familiar with this technology as it is used to create images of a fetus in utero. The technology works because sound waves pass harmlessly and easily through materials of different kinds, but bounce back when they hit a surface of different density from what they are traveling through. (Transmission of sound waves is covered in detail in Chapter 6.) So sound waves travel through the air, but bounce back when they hit a mountainside, creating an echo. They travel through the water, but bounce back when they hit the ocean bottom (or a submarine), creating a sonar image. They travel through the amniotic fluid, but bounce back when they hit a body part. A transducer receives the echoed signal and calculates the time delay between transmission and reception. The time measurement is converted into distance between the transducer and the reflecting object. Graphing these distances creates an outline, resulting in an image of the object being studied.
In speech science, the sonar probe is held under the chin, so that the sound waves travel up through the tongue. They bounce back when they hit the border between the tongue and the air in the mouth, creating an image of tongue shape. Such an image is seen in Figure 1.2: The shape of the to...

Table of contents