Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Language and Thought
eBook - ePub

Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Language and Thought

Share book
  1. English
  2. ePUB (mobile friendly)
  3. Available on iOS & Android
eBook - ePub

Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Language and Thought

Book details
Book preview
Table of contents
Citations

About This Book

III. Language & Thought: Sharon Thompson-Schill (Volume Editor)

(Topics covered include embodied cognition; discourse and dialogue; reading; creativity; speech production; concepts and categorization; culture and cognition; reasoning; sentence processing; bilingualism; speech perception; spatial cognition; word processing; semantic memory; moral reasoning.)

Frequently asked questions

How do I cancel my subscription?
Simply head over to the account section in settings and click on “Cancel Subscription” - it’s as simple as that. After you cancel, your membership will stay active for the remainder of the time you’ve paid for. Learn more here.
Can/how do I download books?
At the moment all of our mobile-responsive ePub books are available to download via the app. Most of our PDFs are also available to download and we're working on making the final remaining ones downloadable now. Learn more here.
What is the difference between the pricing plans?
Both plans give you full access to the library and all of Perlego’s features. The only differences are the price and subscription period: With the annual plan you’ll save around 30% compared to 12 months on the monthly plan.
What is Perlego?
We are an online textbook subscription service, where you can get access to an entire online library for less than the price of a single book per month. With over 1 million books across 1000+ topics, we’ve got you covered! Learn more here.
Do you support text-to-speech?
Look out for the read-aloud symbol on your next book to see if you can listen to it. The read-aloud tool reads text aloud for you, highlighting the text as it is being read. You can pause it, speed it up and slow it down. Learn more here.
Is Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Language and Thought an online PDF/ePUB?
Yes, you can access Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Language and Thought by in PDF and/or ePUB format, as well as other popular books in Psychologie & Psychologie expérimentale. We have over one million books available in our catalogue for you to explore.

Information

Publisher
Wiley
Year
2018
ISBN
9781119170716

CHAPTER 1
Speech Perception

FRANK EISNER AND JAMES M. MCQUEEN

INTRODUCTION

What Speech Is

Speech is the most acoustically complex type of sound that we regularly encounter in our environment. The complexity of the signal reflects the complexity of the movements that speakers perform with their tongues, lips, jaws, and other articulators in order to generate the sounds coming out of their vocal tract. Figure 1.1 shows two representations of the spoken sentence The sun melted the snow—an oscillogram at the top, showing variation in amplitude, and a spectrogram at the bottom, showing its spectral characteristics over time. The figure illustrates some of the richness of the information contained in the speech signal: There are modulations of amplitude, detailed spectral structures, noises, silences, bursts, and sweeps. Some of this structure is relevant in short temporal windows at the level of individual phonetic segments. For example, the vowel in the word sun is characterized by a certain spectral profile, in particular the location of peaks in the spectrum (called “formants,” the darker areas in the spectrogram). Other structures are relevant at the level of words or phrases. For example, the end of the utterance is characterized by a fall in amplitude and in pitch, which spans several segments. The acoustic cues that describe the identity of segments such as individual vowels and consonants are referred to as segmental information, whereas the cues that span longer stretches of the signal such as pitch and amplitude envelope and that signal prosodic structures such as syllables, feet, and intonational phrases are called suprasegmental.
Illustration of Oscillogram (top) and spectrogram (bottom) representations of the speech signal in the sentence “The sun melted the snow.”
Figure 1.1 Oscillogram (top) and spectrogram (bottom) representations of the speech signal in the sentence “The sun melted the snow,” spoken by a male British English speaker. The vertical lines represent approximate phoneme boundaries with phoneme transcriptions in the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) system. The oscillogram shows variation in amplitude (vertical axis) over time (horizontal axis). The spectrogram shows variation in the frequency spectrum (vertical axis) over time (horizontal axis); higher energy in a given part of the spectrum is represented by darker shading.
Acoustic cues are transient and come in fast. The sentence in Figure 1.1 is spoken at a normal speech rate; it contains five syllables and is only 1.3 seconds long. The average duration of a syllable in the sentence is about 260 ms, meaning that information about syllable identity comes in on average at a rate of about 4 Hz, which is quite stable across languages (Giraud & Poeppel, 2012). In addition to the linguistic information that is densely packed in the speech signal, the signal also contains a great deal of additional information about the speaker, the so-called paralinguistic content of speech. If we were to listen to a recording of this sentence, we would be able to say with a fairly high degree of certainty that the speaker is a British middle-aged man with an upper-class accent, and we might also be able to guess that he is suffering from a cold and perhaps is slightly bored as he recorded the prescribed phrase. Paralinguistic information adds to the complexity of speech, and in some cases interacts with how linguistic information is interpreted by listeners (Mullennix & Pisoni, 1990).

What Speech Perception Entails

How, then, is this complex signal perceived? In our view, speech perception is not primarily about how listeners identify individual speech segments (vowels and consonants), though of course this is an important part of the process. Speech perception is also not primarily about how listeners identify suprasegmental units such as syllables and lexical stress patterns, though this is an often overlooked part of the process, too. Ultimately, speech perception is about how listeners use combined sources of segmental and suprasegmental information to recognize spoken words. This is because the listener's goal is to grasp what a speaker means, and the only way she or he can do so is through recognizing the individual meaning units in the speaker's utterance: its morphemes and words. Perceiving segments and prosodic structures is thus at the service of word recognition.
The nature of the speech signal poses a number of computational problems that the listener has to solve in order to be able to recognize spoken words (cf. Marr, 1982). First, listeners have to be able to recognize words in spite of considerable variability in the signal. The oscillogram and spectrogram in Figure 1.1 would look very different if the phrase had been spoken by a female adolescent speaking spontaneously in a casual conversation on a mobile phone in a noisy ski lift, and yet the same words would need to be recognized. Indeed, even if the same speaker recorded the same sentence a second time, it would be physically different (e.g., a different speaking rate, or a different fundamental frequency).
Due to coarticulation (the vocal tract changing both as a consequence of previous articulations and in preparation for upcoming articulations), the acoustic realization of any given segment can be strongly colored by its neighboring segments. There is thus no one-to-one mapping between the perception of a speech sound and its acoustics. This is one of the main factors that is still holding back automatic speech recognition systems (Benzeghiba et al., 2007). In fact, the perceptual system has to solve a many-to-many mapping problem, because not only do instances of the same speech sound have different acoustic properties, but the same acoustic pattern can result in perceiving different speech sounds, depending on the context in which the pattern occurs (Nusbaum & Magnuson, 1997; Repp & Liberman, 1987). The surrounding context of a set of acoustic cues thus has important implications on how the pattern should be interpreted by the listener.
There are also continuous speech processes through which sounds are added (a process called epenthesis), reduced, deleted, or altered, rendering a given word less like its canonical pronunciation. One example of such a process is given in Figure 1.1: The /n/ of sun is realized more like an [m], through a process called coronal place assimilation whereby the coronal /n/ approximates the labial place of articulation of the following word-initial [m].
Speech recognition needs to be robust in the face of all this variability. As we will argue, listeners appear to solve the variability problem in multiple ways, but in particular through phonological abstraction (i.e., categorizing the signal into prelexical segmental and suprasegmental units prior to lexical access) and through being flexible (i.e., through perceptual learning processes that adapt the mapping of the speech signal onto the mental lexicon in response to particular listening situations).
The listener must also solve the segmentation problem. As Figure 1.1 makes clear, the speech signal has nothing that is the equivalent of the white spaces between printed words as in a text such as this that reliably mark where words begin and end. In order to recognize speech, therefore, listeners have to segment the quasicontinuous input stream into discrete words. As with variability, there is no single solution to the segmentation problem: Listeners use multiple cues, and multiple algorithms.
A third problem derives from the fact that, across the world's languages, large lexica (on the order of perhaps 50,000 words) are built from small phonological inventories (on the order of 40 segments in a language such as English, and often much fewer than that; Ladefoged & Maddieson, 1996). Spoken words thus necessarily sound like other spoken words: They begin like other words, they end like other words, and they often have other words partially or wholly embedded within them. This means that, at any moment in the temporal unfolding of an utterance, the signal is likely to be partially or wholly consistent with many words. Once again, the listener appears to solve this “lexical embedding” problem using multiple algorithms.
We will argue that speech perception is based on several stages of processing at which a variety of perceptual operations help the listener solve these three major computational challenges—the variability problem, the segmentation problem, and the lexical embedding problem (see Box 1.1). These stages and operations have been studied over the past 70 years or so using behavioral techniques (e.g., psychophysical tasks such as identification and discrimination; psycholinguistic procedures such as lexical decision, cross-modal priming, and visual-world eye tracking); and neuroscientific techniques (especially measures using electroencephalography [EEG] and magnetoencephalography [MEG]). Neuroimaging techniques (primarily functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI]) and neuropsychological approaches (based on aphasic patients) have also made it possible to start to map these stages of processing onto brain regions. In the following section we will review data of all these different types. These data have made it possible to specify at least three core stages of processing involved in speech perception and the kinds of operations involved at each stage. The data also provide some suggestions about the neural instantiation of these stages.

Box 1.1 Three Com...

Table of contents